1.1 social influence Flashcards
(104 cards)
conformity
a form of social influence in which one adjusts their behaviour or thinking as a result of real or imagined pressure from a group of people
types of conformity
kelman (1958): compliance, internalisation and identification
compliance
compliance is when individuals adjust the behaviour, attitudes or beliefs they show in public, so that they are in line with the majority.
there is no change to privately held behaviour, attitudes or beliefs and conformity only lasts while the group is present.
it is therefore a superficial and temporary form of conformity.
> eg. student pretending to find conversations interesting on their first day to fit in even though they find it incredibly dull
internalisation
internalisation is when individuals adjust their behaviour, attitudes or beliefs publicly AND privately so that they are in line with the majority.
the individual examines their own behaviour and attitudes based on what others are saying and decide that the majority is correct.
this is deeper than compliance and a more permanent form of conformity.
> eg. a student watching other students closely on their first day of school, and eventually dressing and behaving like then because they agree and value their dress sense and behaviour
identification
identification is when an individual accepts social influence because they want to be associated with a role model or a social group.
by adopting the role model/group’s behaviour, attitudes or beliefs they feel connected to the role model/group, even though we don’t necessarily agree with everything the majority believes
> eg. student looks up to another group of teenagers/admires them, so decides that they want to be like them and associated with them so they alter their behaviour to be in line with the majority
explanations for conformity
deutsch and gerard (1955) developed a two process theory for the two reasons for conformity - normative social influence (the desire to be LIKED/accepted) and informational social influence (the desire to be RIGHT/sure)
informational social influence
people conform if they are unsure of the correct answer or how to behave in social situations and want to be right along with the group.
the drive for conformity is often sensible. if the majority is right, they are too and if they are not, at least they won’t stand out.
> eg. in a classroom where a student doesn’t know the answer they may turn to those who they think know better
ISI more likely + leads to
ISI leads to internalisation, where people publically AND privately change their opinions.
ISI is more likely to happen if the situation is ambiguous (the correct answer is not clear), a crisis (rapid action) or when others are more expert.
normative social influence
people conform because they have a fundamental need to be liked and accepted by others in a group.
we avoid behaviour that will lead to rejection or ridicule, which leads to copying others behaviour to ‘fit in’.
research shows that people like people who are similar to them, so conformity can be an effective strategy to ensure acceptance into a group.
NSI more likely + leads to
NSI leads to compliance, where people will agree publicly with the group but privately they do not change their personal opinions.
NSI is most likely to occur in situations with strangers (avoiding rejection) or stressful situations (greater need for social support)
strength of explanations of conformity (NSI)
+ asch (1951) asked participants to say which of three ‘test lines’ was the same as the ‘standard line’.
the participants were in a group with confederates who purposefully gave the same wrong answer, even though the correct answer was obvious.
in 35% of the trials, the participants conformed to the group and gave the wrong answer (the chance of making a genuine mistake on this task was only 1%). they conformed due to normative social influence.
after the experiment they claimed that they knew the correct answer but were worried that the group would ridicule them if they answered differently to everyone else.
strength of explanations of conformity (ISI)
+ lucas et al (2006) asked students to give answers to mathematical problems that were easy or more difficult.
he found higher conformity rates to incorrect answers in the more difficult questions rather than easy ones. this was especially true for those students who felt that their mathematical skills were poor.
this study supports ISI because it suggests that conformity is more likely to occur when the situation is ambiguous or difficult. in the study, those students who were poor at maths looked to others who they felt had more knowledge and were right.
weakness of explanations of conformity (NSI)
- there are individual differences that this explanation does not account for. not every individual shows NSI.
individuals who are not concerned about being liked are less affected by NSI, but ‘nAffiliators’, for example, are. these are people who have a greater need to be liked and are more likely to conform (mcghee and teevan 1967).
this suggests that the NSI explanation for conformity may lack population validity (it doesn’t apply to everyone).
weakness of explanations of conformity (ISI)
- similarly, ISI does not affect everyone’s behaviour in the same way. for example, asch (1955) found that students were not as conformist (28%) as other types of participants (37%), like those who work in offices. this suggests that even if the situation may be ambiguous we don’t always look to others for support and will not always conform.
weakness of explanations of conformity: one concept
- we can see NSI and ISI as one concept on why we conform rather than two. for example, in asch’s experiments, conformity was reduced when there was one other dissenting participant.
the dissenter may have reduced the power of NSI because they provided social support to the participant.
or, they may have reduced the power of ISI by giving the participant an alternative source of information.
this shows that it is not always clear whether it is NSI or ISI at work in conformity situations.
weakness of explanations of conformity: lab studies
- many supporting studies for NSI and ISI as explanations for conformity are lab studies which lack ecological validity.
for example, asch’s study asked about the length of lines which would never happen in real life, so we don’t know that people will behave the same way in the real world.
jenness (1932)
+ jenness (1932) investigated whether individuals would change their opinions in an ambiguous situation in response to group discussion.
26 students were asked to estimate how many beans they thought were in a jar on their own, in a group of three and then on their own again
he found that nearly all participants changed their original answer when the task was carried out in a group (males by 256 and females by 382 beans).
this is likely to be an example of informational social influence as participants would be uncertain about the actual number of beans in the jar and so be genuinely influenced by the group.
sherif (1935)
+ sherif (1935) used the autokinetic effect to investigate conformity. this is where a small spot of light (projected onto a screen) in a dark room will appear to move, even though it is still (i.e. it is a visual illusion).
it was discovered that when participants were tested individually their estimates of how far the light moved varied considerably (eg. from 20cm to 80cm).
the participants were then tested in groups of three. sherif manipulated the composition of the group by putting together two people whose estimates when alone were very similar, and one person whose estimate was very different.
each person in the group had to say aloud how far they thought the light had moved. sherif found that over numerous estimates of the movement of light, the group converged to a common estimate.
the person whose estimate of movement was greatly different from the other two in the group conformed to the view of the other two because of informational social influence. the task was ambiguous so they looked to others for the answer.
asch (1951) procedure
asch (1951) studied 123 male american undergraduates in a ‘vision test’.
he placed 1 naïve participant in a group with up to 8 other confederates, who had previously agreed their answers to each question (they were only pretending to be participants)
the group was asked to look at a ‘standard line’ and then decide individually which of three other ‘test lines’ were the same length without discussing it with one another.
they gave their responses one at a time out loud, with the participant going last so they heard the rest of the groups’ responses before giving their own.
the answer was obvious, but the confederates gave the wrong answer on 12 of the 18 trials on purpose.
asch (1951) results
the chance of making a genuine mistake on this task was only 1% but 35% of the responses given by participants were incorrect in the critical trials. 75% of participants conformed in at least one of the 18 trials.
when asch interviewed his participants afterwards, he discovered that the majority of participants who had conformed had continued to trust their own judgment but gave the same answer as the group to avoid disapproval (normative social influence).
the study supports the idea of compliance and normative social influence because participants wanted to be LIKED more than RIGHT
asch’s variables affecting conformity
group size, unanimity, task difficulty
group size
asch (1956) increased group size by adding more confederates (increasing the size of the majority).
groups with 1-2 confederates had low conformity rates, but this went up to around 30% with 3 confederates.
this shows that conformity occurs up to a certain point, as rates of conformity leveled off when the majority was greater than 3.
group size: campbell and fairey
campbell and fairey (1989) said group size is affected depending on the JUDGEMENT being made and the MOTIVATION of the individual. ambiguous situations mean that people conform to follow the majority, otherwise its to fit in.
unanimity
when the group had unanimity (everyone agreed) conformity increased.
however, when there was at least one dissenter (meaning that the group was not unanimous in their answer), conformity dropped.
asch (1956) found that even the presence of just one confederate who went against the majority reduced conformity from 33% to 5%.
even when the confederate gave a different wrong answer to the rest of the group conformity dropped from 33% to 9%.