Unit 6: Social Behavior Flashcards

1
Q

Solomon Asch

A

PERSPECTIVE ON CONFORMITY
In some of the classic research into impression formation, Solomon Asch demonstrated the importance that what he called central traits can have on the impressions we form of others. When you interact with people, you’re constantly engaged in person perception, the process of forming impressions of others. People show considerable ingenuity in piecing together clues about others’ characteristics. However, impressions are often inaccurate because of the many biases and fallacies that occur in person perception.
Asch devised a clever procedure that reduced ambiguity about whether subjects were conforming, allowing him to investigate the variables that govern conformity. Let’s re-create one of Asch’s classic experiments, which have become the most widely replicated studies in the history of social psychology. The subjects are male undergraduates recruited for a study of visual perception. A group of seven subjects is shown a large card with a vertical line on it and the subjects are then asked to indicate which of three lines on a second card matches the original “standard line” in length. All seven subjects are given a turn at the task, and they announce their choice to the group. The subject in the sixth chair doesn’t know it, but everyone else in the group is an accomplice of the experimenter, and they’re about to make him wonder whether he has taken leave of his senses.
The accomplices give accurate responses on the first two trials. On the third trial, line 2 clearly is the correct response, but the first five “subjects” all say that line 3 matches the standard line. The genuine subject is bewildered and can’t believe his ears. Over the course of the next 15 trials, the accomplices all give the same incorrect response on 11 of them. How does the real subject respond? The line judgments are easy and unambiguous. So, if the participant consistently agrees with the accomplices, he isn’t making honest mistakes—he’s conforming.
Averaging across all 50 participants, Asch found that the young men conformed on 37 percent of the trials. The subjects varied considerably in their tendency to conform, however. Of the 50 participants, 13 never caved into the group, while 14 conformed on more than half of the trials. One could argue that the results show that people confronting a unanimous majority generally tend to resist the pressure to conform. However, given how clear and easy the line judgments were, most social scientists viewed the findings as a dramatic demonstration of humans’ propensity to conform.
In subsequent studies, Asch found that group size and group unanimity are key determinants of conformity. To examine the impact of group size, Asch repeated his procedure with groups that included from 1 to 15 accomplices. Little conformity was seen when a subject was pitted against just one person, but conformity increased rapidly as group size went from two to four, and then leveled off. Thus, Asch reasoned that as groups grow larger, conformity increases—up to a point, a conclusion that has been echoed by other researchers.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Elaine Hatfield and Ellen Berscheid

A

Two early pioneers in research on love were Elaine Hatfield and Ellen Berscheid. They have proposed that romantic relationships are characterized by two kinds of love: passionate love and companionate love. Passionate love is a complete absorption in another that includes tender sexual feelings and the agony and ecstasy of intense emotion. Companionate love is warm, trusting, tolerant affection for another whose life is deeply intertwined with one’s own. Passionate and companionate love can co-exist. They don’t, however, necessarily go hand in hand. Initially, it was thought that passionate love peaks in intensity early in relationships and then declines significantly over time. However, more recent research suggests that in relationships that remain intact, the erosion of passionate love tends to be gradual and modest, with levels remaining fairly high in most couples .
Research demonstrates that passionate love is a powerful motivational force that produces profound changes in people’s thinking, emotion, and behavior. Interestingly, brain-imaging research indicates that when people think about someone they are passionately in love with, these thoughts light up the dopamine circuits in the brain that are known to be activated by cocaine and other addictive drugs. Perhaps that explains why passionate love sometimes resembles an addiction.
Passionate and companionate love may co-exist, but they don’t necessarily go hand in hand. Research suggests that, as a general rule, companionate love is more strongly related to relationship satisfaction than passionate love. The distinction between passionate and companionate love has been further refined by Robert Sternberg. He subdivides companionate love into intimacy and commitment. Intimacy refers to warmth, closeness, and sharing in a relationship. Commitment is an intent to maintain a relationship in spite of the difficulties and costs that may arise.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

John Cacioppo

A

The elaboration likelihood model of attitude change, originally proposed by Richard Petty and John Cacioppo, asserts that there are two basic “routes” to persuasion. The central route is taken when people carefully ponder the content and logic of persuasive messages. The peripheral route is taken when persuasion depends on non message factors, such as the attractiveness and credibility of the source, or on conditioned emotional responses. For example, a politician who campaigns by delivering carefully researched speeches that thoughtfully analyze complex issues is following the central route to persuasion. In contrast, a politician who depends on marching bands, flag-waving, celebrity endorsements, and emotional slogans is following the peripheral route.
Both routes can lead to persuasion. However, according to the elaboration likelihood model, the durability of attitude change depends on the extent to which people elaborate on (think about) the contents of persuasive communications. Studies suggest that the central route to persuasion leads to more enduring attitude change than the peripheral route. Research also suggests that attitudes changed through central processes predict behavior better than attitudes changed through peripheral processes.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

William Cunningham

A

William Cunningham, a faculty member at the University of Toronto, and his colleagues, have examined a variety of social psychological topics from a social neuroscience perspective. In one study they used the characteristics of implicit and explicit evaluations to explore the role of the amygdala in people’s responses to white and black faces. Previous neuroscience research had often implicated the amygdala in fear responses, and other social psychological research had shown that whites frequently show more negative evaluations of blacks than of whites. During the fMRI, white participants were presented with various stimuli, including neutral-expression black and white faces. The stimuli were presented briefly (30 milliseconds) or for a longer duration (525 milliseconds). Cunningham used the shorter presentation time to assess automatic responses to the stimuli, and the longer presentation times to assess controlled, conscious evaluations of the stimuli.
Cunningham expected that his white participants would show greater activation in the amygdala when presented with black faces, most notably under brief presentation times, when automatic responses were being assessed, and this is just what the result of his experiments revealed. In addition, Cunningham found that this heightened activation of the amygdala was especially true for participants that he had previously identified as being more racially biased. This and other similar research suggests that “implicit associations to a social group may result in automatic emotional response when encountering members of that group”. Findings of greater amygdala response to black faces using fMRI methods is common in the literature. Other parallel research in the area has employed ERP techniques. At this point, it is unclear whether these effects reflect negative evaluations of black faces or are the result of greater “perceptual” expertise on the part of white participants for the faces of ingroup (i.e., white) members. Considerable research effort is currently being devoted to these and other questions in the neuropsychology of stereotypes and prejudice.
While the social neuroscience approach has clear limitations, we believe it will increasingly contribute to our understanding of human behavior in a social context and that the range of the phenomena to which it is applied will continue to expand. As neuroscientists continue to explore ways of integrating models of neuroscience with models of social psychology, we expect the literature to offer more, and more refined, explanations that contribute to our understanding of ourselves and others.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Leon Festinger

A

Leon Festinger’s dissonance theory assumes that inconsistency among attitudes propels people in the direction of attitude change. Dissonance theory had a profound impact on the directions taken by researchers in social psychology, and in other disciplines. It burst into prominence in 1959 when Festinger and J. Merrill Carlsmith published a famous study of counterattitudinal behavior. Let’s look at their findings and at how dissonance theory explains them.
Festinger and Carlsmith had male college students come to a laboratory, where they worked on excruciatingly dull tasks such as turning pegs repeatedly. When a subject’s hour was over, the experimenter confided that some participants’ motivation was being manipulated by telling them that the task was interesting and enjoyable before they started it. Then, after a moment’s hesitation, the experimenter asked if the subject could help him out of a jam. His usual helper was delayed and he needed someone to testify to the next “subject” (really an accomplice) that the experimental task was interesting. He offered to pay the subject if he would tell the person in the adjoining waiting room that the task was enjoyable and involving.
This entire scenario was enacted to coax participants into doing something that was inconsistent with their true feelings—that is, to engage in counterattitudinal behavior. Some participants received a token payment of $1 for their effort, while others received a more substantial payment of $20 (an amount equivalent to about $164 today, in light of inflation). Later, a second experimenter inquired about the subjects’ true feelings regarding the dull experimental task.
Who do you think rated the task more favorably—the subjects who were paid $1 or those who were paid $20? Both common sense and learning theory would predict that the subjects who received the greater reward ($20) should come to like the task more. In reality, however, the subjects who were paid $1 exhibited more favorable attitude change—just as Festinger and Carlsmith had predicted. Why? Dissonance theory provides an explanation.
According to Festinger, cognitive dissonance exists when related cognitions are inconsistent—that is, when they contradict each other. Cognitive dissonance is thought to create an unpleasant state of tension that motivates people to reduce their dissonance—usually by altering their cognitions. In the study by Festinger and Carlsmith, the subjects’ contradictory cognitions were “The task is boring” and “I told someone the task was enjoyable.” The subjects who were paid $20 for lying had an obvious reason for behaving inconsistently with their true attitudes, so these subjects experienced little dissonance. In contrast, the subjects paid $1 had no readily apparent justification for their lie and experienced high dissonance. To reduce it, they tended to persuade themselves that the task was more enjoyable than they had originally thought. Thus, dissonance theory sheds light on why people sometimes come to believe their own lies.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Cindy Hazen and Philip Shaver

A

In a groundbreaking analysis of love, Hazan and Shaver looked at similarities between adult love and attachment relationships in infancy. We noted in Chapter 11 that infant-caregiver bonding, or attachment, emerges in the first year of life. Early attachments vary in quality, and infants tend to fall into three groups. Most infants develop a secure attachment. However, some are very anxious when separated from their caregiver, a syndrome called anxious-ambivalent attachment. A third group of infants, characterized by avoidant attachment, never bond very well with their caregiver.
According to Hazan and Shaver, romantic love is an attachment process, and people’s intimate relationships in adulthood follow the same form as their attachments in infancy. In their theory, a person who had an anxious-ambivalent attachment in infancy will tend to have romantic relations marked by anxiety and ambivalence in adulthood. In other words, people relive their early bonding experiences with their parents in their romantic relationships in adulthood.
Hazan and Shaver’s initial survey study provided striking support for their theory. They found that adults’ love relationships could be sorted into groups that paralleled the three patterns of attachment seen in infants. Secure adults found it relatively easy to get close to others and described their love relations as trusting. Anxious-ambivalent adults reported a preoccupation with love, accompanied by expectations of rejection, and they described their love relations as volatile and marked by jealousy. Avoidant adults found it difficult to get close to others and described their love relations as lacking intimacy and trust. Research eventually showed that attachment patterns are reasonably stable over time and that people’s working models of attachment are carried forward from one relationship to the next. These findings supported the notion that individuals’ infant attachment experiences shape their intimate relations in adulthood.
Research on the correlates of adult attachment styles has grown exponentially since the mid-1990s. Consistent with the original theory, research has shown that securely attached individuals have more committed, satisfying, intimate, well-adjusted, and longer-lasting relationships than do people with anxious-ambivalent or avoidant attachment styles. Moreover, studies have shown that people with different attachment styles are predisposed to think, feel, and behave differently in their relationships. For example, people high in attachment anxiety tend to behave in awkward ways that undermine their dating success. Worried about the likelihood of rejection, they end up courting rejection by acting cold, wary, disengaged, and preoccupied with themselves. When they do get involved in romantic relationships, people high in attachment anxiety tend to overreact emotionally to conflict with their partners. Their exaggerated expressions of hurt and vulnerability are designed to make their partners feel guilty, but these manipulative efforts end up having a negative impact on their relationship.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Fritz Heider

A

Fritz Heider was the first to describe how people make attributions. He asserted that people tend to locate the cause of behavior either within a person, attributing it to personal factors, or outside a person, attributing it to environmental factors.
Elaborating on Heider’s insight, various theorists have agreed that explanations of behavior and events can be categorized as internal or external attributions. Internal attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to personal dispositions, traits, abilities, and feelings. External attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to situational demands and environmental constraints. For example, if a friend’s business fails, you might attribute it to his or her lack of business acumen (an internal, personal factor) or to negative trends in the nation’s economic climate (an external, situational explanation). Parents who find out that their teenage son has just banged up the car may blame it on his carelessness (a personal disposition) or on slippery road conditions (a situational factor).
Internal and external attributions can have a tremendous impact on everyday interpersonal interactions. Blaming a friend’s business failure on poor business acumen as opposed to a poor economy will have a great impact on how you view your friend. Likewise, if parents attribute their son’s automobile accident to slippery road conditions, they’re likely to deal with the event very differently than if they attribute it to his carelessness.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Irving Janis

A

DESCRIBED GROUPTHINK
Irving Janis first described groupthink in his effort to explain how former U.S. president John F. Kennedy and his advisors could have miscalculated so badly in deciding to invade Cuba at the Bay of Pigs in 1961. The attempted invasion failed miserably and, in retrospect, seemed remarkably ill-conceived.
Applying his many years of research and theory on group dynamics to the Bay of Pigs fiasco, Janis developed a model of groupthink. When groups get caught up in groupthink, members suspend their critical judgment and the group starts censoring dissent as the pressure to conform increases. Soon, everyone begins to think alike. Moreover, some members serve as “mind guards” and try to shield the group from information that contradicts the group’s view.
If the group’s view is challenged from outside, victims of groupthink tend to think in simplistic “us versus them” terms. Members begin to overestimate the ingroup’s unanimity, and they begin to view the outgroup as the enemy. Groupthink also promotes incomplete gathering of information. Like individuals, groups often display a confirmation bias, as they tend to seek and focus on information that supports their initial views.
Recent research has uncovered another factor that may contribute to groupthink—individual members often fail to share information that is unique to them. Sound decision making depends on group members combining their information effectively. However, when groups discuss issues, they have an interesting tendency to focus mainly on the information that the members already share as opposed to encouraging offers of information unique to individual members. Additional research is needed to determine why groups are mediocre at pooling members’ information.
What causes groupthink? According to Janis, a key precondition is high group cohesiveness. Group cohesiveness refers to the strength of the liking relationships linking group members to each other and to the group itself. Members of cohesive groups are close-knit, are committed, have “team spirit,” and are very loyal to the group. Cohesiveness itself isn’t bad. It can facilitate group productivity and help groups achieve great things. But Janis maintains that the danger of groupthink is greater when groups are highly cohesive. Groupthink is also more likely when a group works in relative isolation, when the group’s power structure is dominated by a strong, directive leader, and when the group is under stress to make a major decision. Under these conditions, group discussions can easily lead to group polarization, strengthening the group’s dominant view.
A relatively small number of experiments have been conducted to test Janis’s theory, because the antecedent conditions thought to foster groupthink—such as high decision stress, strong group cohesiveness, and dominating leadership—are difficult to create effectively in laboratory settings. The evidence on groupthink consists mostly of retrospective case studies of major decision-making fiascos. So, Janis’s model of groupthink should probably be characterized as an innovative, sophisticated, intuitively appealing theory that needs to be subjected to much more empirical study.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Dennis Krebs

A

EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON PERSON PERCEPTION
Why is the process of person perception riddled with bias? Evolutionary psychologists like Simon Fraser University’s Dennis Krebs argue that some of the biases seen in social perception were adaptive in humans’ ancestral environment. For example, they argue that person perception is swayed by physical attractiveness because attractiveness was associated with reproductive potential in women and with health, vigor, and the accumulation of material resources in men.
What about the human tendency to automatically categorize others? Evolutionary theorists attribute this behavior to our distant ancestors’ need to quickly separate friend from foe. They assert that humans are programmed by evolution to immediately classify people as members of an ingroup—a group that one belongs to and identifies with, or as members of an outgroup—a group that one does not belong to or identify with. This crucial categorization is thought to structure subsequent perceptions. As Krebs and Denton put it, “It is as though the act of classifying others as ingroup or outgroup members activates two quite different brain circuits”. Ingroup members tend to be viewed in a favorable light, whereas outgroup members tend to be viewed in terms of various negative stereotypes. According to Krebs and Denton, these negative stereotypes (“They are inferior; they are all alike; they will exploit us”) move outgroups out of our domain of empathy, so we feel justified in not liking them or in discriminating against them, or even in some circumstances dehumanizing them.
Evolutionary psychologists, then, ascribe much of the bias in person perception to cognitive mechanisms that have been shaped by natural selection.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Stanley Milgram

A

Stanley Milgram wanted to study this tendency to obey authority figures. Like many other people after World War II, he was troubled by how readily the citizens of Germany had followed the orders of dictator Adolf Hitler, even when the orders required morally repugnant actions, such as the slaughter of millions of Jews. Milgram, who had worked with Solomon Asch, set out to design a standard laboratory procedure for the study of obedience, much like Asch’s procedure for studying conformity. The clever experiment that Milgram devised became one of the most famous and controversial studies in the annals of psychology. It has been hailed as a “monumental contribution” to science and condemned as “dangerous, dehumanizing, and unethical research”.
Milgram’s participants were a diverse collection of 40 men from the local community. They were told that they would be participating in a study concerned with the effects of punishment on learning. When they arrived at the lab, they drew slips of paper from a hat to get their assignments. The drawing was rigged so that the subject always became the “teacher” and an experimental accomplice became the “learner.”
In the research, participants were induced to use shocks on learners in a learning experiment as feedback when the learners failed at a rigged task. While the learners were in fact confederates and the shocks were nonexistent, participants believed that they had delivered potentially harmful shocks to another person because they were told to do so by an authority figure, the experimenter. In the experiment, participants were induced to increase the shocks to what they believed would be dangerous levels as the learner continued to make mistakes in his task performance. Most participants went ahead and increased the shocks. Decades after the research was conducted, it still generates spirited debate.
After his initial demonstration, Milgram tried about 20 variations on his experimental procedure, looking for factors that influence participants’ obedience. In one variation, Milgram moved the study away from Yale’s campus to see if the prestige of the university was contributing to the subjects’ obedience. When the study was run in a seedy office building by the “Research Associates of Bridgeport,” only a small decrease in obedience was observed (48 percent of the subjects gave all of the shocks). Even when the learner was in the same room with the subjects, 40 percent of the participants administered the full series of shocks. As a whole, Milgram was surprised at how high subjects’ obedience remained as he changed various aspects of his experiment.
That said, there were some situational manipulations that reduced obedience appreciably. For example, if the authority figure was called away and the orders were given by an ordinary person (supposedly another participant), full obedience dropped to 20 percent. In another version of the study, Milgram borrowed a trick from Asch’s conformity experiments and set up teams of three teachers that included two more accomplices. When they drew lots, the real subject was always selected to run the shock apparatus in consultation with the other two “teachers.” When both accomplices accepted the experimenter’s orders to continue shocking the learner, the pressure increased obedience a bit. However, if an accomplice defied the experimenter and supported the subject’s objections, obedience declined dramatically (only 10 percent of the subjects gave all the shocks), just as conformity had dropped rapidly when dissent surfaced in Asch’s conformity studies. These findings are interesting in that they provide further support for Milgram’s thesis that situational factors exert great influence over behavior. If the situational pressures favoring obedience are decreased, obedience declines, as one would expect.
Milgram’s study evoked a controversy that continues to the present. According to Murray Goddard of the University of New Brunswick, some of the hesitation to accept Milgram’s findings may have resulted from the fact that they were counter to human intuition. Other critics have argued that Milgram’s results can’t be generalized to apply to the real world. They maintain that the participants went along only because they knew it was an experiment and “everything must be okay.” And some have argued that subjects who agree to participate in a scientific study expect to obey orders from an experimenter. Milgram replied by arguing that if subjects had thought, “everything must be okay,” they wouldn’t have experienced the enormous distress that they clearly showed.
As for the idea that research participants expect to follow an experimenter’s commands, Milgram pointed out that so do real-world soldiers and bureaucrats who are accused of villainous acts performed in obedience to authority. “I reject Baumrind’s argument that the observed obedience doesn’t count because it occurred where it is appropriate,” said Milgram. “That is precisely why it does count.” Overall, the evidence supports the generalizability of Milgram’s results, which were consistently replicated for many years, in diverse settings, with a variety of subjects and procedural variations.
Critics also questioned the ethics of Milgram’s procedure. They noted that without prior consent, subjects were exposed to extensive deception that could undermine their trust in people and to severe stress that could leave emotional scars. Moreover, most participants also had to confront the disturbing fact that they caved in to the experimenter’s commands to inflict harm on an innocent victim.
Milgram’s defenders argued that the brief distress experienced by his subjects was a small price to pay for the insights that emerged from his obedience studies. Looking back, however, many psychologists seem to share the critics’ concerns about the ethical implications of Milgram’s work. His procedure is questionable by contemporary standards of research ethics, and no replications of his obedience study have been conducted in the United States from the mid-1970s until recently, when Jerry Burger crafted a very cautious, partial replication that incorporated a variety of additional safeguards to protect the welfare of the participants.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Robert Sternberg

A

The distinction between passionate and companionate love has been further refined by Robert Sternberg. He subdivides companionate love into intimacy and commitment. Intimacy refers to warmth, closeness, and sharing in a relationship. Commitment is an intent to maintain a relationship in spite of the difficulties and costs that may arise.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Bernard Weiner

A

After studying the attributions that people make in explaining success and failure, Bernard Weiner (1980, 1986, 1994, 2004) concluded that people often focus on the stability of the causes underlying behavior. According to Weiner, the stable–unstable dimension in attribution cuts across the internal–external dimension, creating four types of attributions for success and failure.
Let’s apply Weiner’s model to a concrete event. Imagine that you’re contemplating why you failed to get a job that you wanted. You might attribute your setback to internal factors that are stable (lack of ability) or unstable (inadequate effort to put together an eye-catching résumé). Or you might attribute your setback to external factors that are stable (too much outstanding competition) or unstable (bad luck). If you got the job, your explanations for your success would fall into the same four categories: internal–stable (your excellent ability), internal–unstable (your hard work to assemble a superb résumé), external–stable (lack of top-flight competition), and external–unstable (good luck).
Weiner’s model can be used to understand complex issues in the real world. For example, when people analyze the causes of poverty, their explanations tend to fit neatly into the cells of Weiner’s model: internal-stable (laziness, lack of thrift); internal-unstable (financially draining illness); external-stable (discrimination, inadequate government programs for training); and external-unstable (bad luck, economic recession).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Mark Zanna

A

Our perception of others is also subject to self-fulfilling prophecy; in effect, creating what we expect to see. This was clearly demonstrated in a classic study by Mark Zanna and his colleagues. The study was designed to show the operation of self-fulfilling prophecy. If you hold strong beliefs about the characteristics of another group, you may behave in such a way so as to bring about these characteristics. The research had two studies. In the first study, researchers had white undergraduate males interview either a black or white job applicant. The applicant was, in fact, an experimental accomplice or confederate. It was found that when the job applicant was black, the interviewers tended to sit farther away, end the interview more quickly, and make more speech errors (e.g., stuttering, stammering). Clearly, then, the white interviewers changed how they acted depending on the race of the interviewee. In interviewing a white accomplice, they adopted what was referred to as an immediate style (i.e., sitting closer, more eye contact), but when they interviewed a black accomplice they used a nonimmediate style (i.e., sitting farther away, making more speech errors, looking away).
In the second study, Word, Zanna, and Cooper attempted to find out how it would feel to have someone behave toward you in a nonimmediate style. In the study, white experimental accomplices interviewed other white students while adopting either the immediate or nonimmediate style. Students who had been interviewed in the nonimmediate style seemed more anxious and did not perform as well in the interview.
When we think about the effects of stereotypes, we often focus on the effects our stereotypes have on others and how self-fulfilling prophecy processes might serve to confirm those stereotypes. But, of course, our stereotypes also affect us; they influence our conceptualizations of our social environment. But the influence of our stereotypes on us doesn’t end there; they can also directly affect our own behavior.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Attitudes

A

Attitudes are positive or negative evaluations of objects of thought. “Objects of thought” may include social issues (capital punishment or gun control, for example), groups (liberals, farmers), institutions (the Lutheran Church, the Supreme Court), consumer products (yogurt, computers), and people (the prime minister, your next-door neighbor).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Attributions

A

Attributions are inferences that people draw about the causes of events, others’ behavior, and their own behavior. If you conclude that a friend turned down your invitation because she’s overworked, you have made an attribution about the cause of her behavior (and, implicitly, have rejected other possible explanations). If you conclude that you’re stuck at home with nothing to do because you failed to plan ahead, you’ve made an attribution about the cause of an event (being stuck at home). If you conclude that you failed to plan ahead because you’re a procrastinator, you’ve made an attribution about the cause of your own behavior. People make attributions mainly because they have a strong need to understand their experiences. They want to make sense out of their own behavior, others’ actions, and the events in their lives. In this section, we’ll take a look at some of the patterns seen when people make attributions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Bystander Effect

A

When it comes to helping behavior, many studies have uncovered an apparent paradox called the bystander effect: people are less likely to provide needed help when they are in groups than when they are alone.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Channel

A

The process of persuasion includes an examination of factors that affect persuasion, with research in the area emphasizing four basic elements: source, receiver, message, and channel. The source is the person who sends a communication, and the receiver is the person to whom the message is sent. So, if you watch a political news conference on TV, the politician is the source, and you and millions of other viewers are the receivers. The message is the information transmitted by the source, and the channel is the medium through which the message is sent.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Cognitive Dissonance

A

According to Festinger, cognitive dissonance exists when related cognitions are inconsistent—that is, when they contradict each other. Cognitive dissonance is thought to create an unpleasant state of tension that motivates people to reduce their dissonance—usually by altering their cognitions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Collectivism

A

Decades of research have shown that cultural differences in individualism versus collectivism influence attributional tendencies as well as many other aspects of social behavior. Individualism involves putting personal goals ahead of group goals and defining one’s identity in terms of personal attributes rather than group memberships. In contrast, collectivism involves putting group goals ahead of personal goals and defining one’s identity in terms of the groups one belongs to (such as one’s family, tribe, work group, social class, and caste).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Commitment

A

The distinction between passionate and companionate love has been further refined by Robert Sternberg. He subdivides companionate love into intimacy and commitment. Intimacy refers to warmth, closeness, and sharing in a relationship. Commitment is an intent to maintain a relationship in spite of the difficulties and costs that may arise.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Companionate Love

A

Two early pioneers in research on love were Elaine Hatfield and Ellen Berscheid. They have proposed that romantic relationships are characterized by two kinds of love: passionate love and companionate love. Passionate love is a complete absorption in another that includes tender sexual feelings and the agony and ecstasy of intense emotion. Companionate love is warm, trusting, tolerant affection for another whose life is deeply intertwined with one’s own. Passionate and companionate love can co-exist. They don’t, however, necessarily go hand in hand.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Conformity

A

Conformity occurs when people yield to real or imagined social pressure. For example, if you maintain a well-groomed lawn only to avoid complaints from your neighbors, you’re yielding to social pressure. If you like Rage Against the Machine because you genuinely enjoy their music, that’s not conformity. However, if you like Rage because doing so is “cool” and your friends would question your taste if you didn’t, then you’re conforming.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Defensive Attribution

A

The defensive attribution is the tendency to blame victims for their misfortunes, so that one feels less likely to be victimized in a similar way. The defensive attribution is relevant to situations in which you are attempting to explain calamities and setbacks that befall others. Here, an observer’s tendency to make internal attributions becomes even stronger than normal. Blaming a victim helps people maintain their belief in a just world, where they are unlikely to suffer a similar fate.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Discrimination

A

Prejudice may lead to discrimination, which involves behaving differently, usually unfairly, toward the members of a group. Prejudice and discrimination tend to go hand in hand, but attitudes and behavior do not necessarily correspond.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Elaboration Likelihood Model

A

The elaboration likelihood model of attitude change, originally proposed by Richard Petty and John Cacioppo, asserts that there are two basic “routes” to persuasion. The central route is taken when people carefully ponder the content and logic of persuasive messages. The peripheral route is taken when persuasion depends on non message factors, such as the attractiveness and credibility of the source, or on conditioned emotional responses. For example, a politician who campaigns by delivering carefully researched speeches that thoughtfully analyze complex issues is following the central route to persuasion. In contrast, a politician who depends on marching bands, flag-waving, celebrity endorsements, and emotional slogans is following the peripheral route.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

Ethnocentrism

A

Ethnocentrism is a tendency to view one’s own group as superior to others and as the standard for judging the worth of foreign ways.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

Explicit Attitudes

A

Explicit attitudes are attitudes that we hold consciously and can readily describe. For the most part, these overt attitudes are what social psychologists have always studied until fairly recently.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

External Attributions

A

External attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to situational demands and environmental constraints. For example, if a friend’s business fails, you might attribute it to his or her lack of business acumen (an internal, personal factor) or to negative trends in the nation’s economic climate (an external, situational explanation). Parents who find out that their teenage son has just banged up the car may blame it on his carelessness (a personal disposition) or on slippery road conditions (a situational factor).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

Foot-in-the-Door Technique

A

The foot-in-the-door technique involves getting people to agree to a small request to increase the chances that they will agree to a larger request later. This technique is widely used in all walks of life. For example, groups seeking donations often ask people to simply sign a petition first.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

Fundamental Attribution Error

A

A common form of bias seen in observers is the fundamental attribution error, which refers to observers’ bias in favor of internal attributions in explaining others’ behavior.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

Group

A

In social psychologists’ eyes, a group consists of two or more individuals who interact and are interdependent. Historically, most groups have interacted on a face-to-face basis, but advances in telecommunications are rapidly changing that situation. In the era of the Internet, people can interact, become interdependent, and develop a group identity without ever meeting in person.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

Group Cohesiveness

A

What causes groupthink? According to Janis, a key precondition is high group cohesiveness. Group cohesiveness refers to the strength of the liking relationships linking group members to each other and to the group itself. Members of cohesive groups are close-knit, are committed, have “team spirit,” and are very loyal to the group. Cohesiveness itself isn’t bad. It can facilitate group productivity and help groups achieve great things. But Janis maintains that the danger of groupthink is greater when groups are highly cohesive. Groupthink is also more likely when a group works in relative isolation, when the group’s power structure is dominated by a strong, directive leader, and when the group is under stress to make a major decision. Under these conditions, group discussions can easily lead to group polarization, strengthening the group’s dominant view.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

Group Polarization

A

Investigators eventually determined that groups can shift either way, toward risk or caution, depending on which way the group is leaning to begin with. A shift toward a more extreme position, an effect called polarization, is often the result of group discussion. Thus, group polarization occurs when group discussion strengthens a group’s dominant point of view and produces a shift toward a more extreme decision in that direction. Group polarization does not involve widening the gap between factions in a group, as its name might suggest. In fact, group polarization can contribute to consensus in a group, as we’ll see in our upcoming discussion of groupthink. Group polarization can occur in all sorts of groups. For example, recent studies have looked at how group polarization plays a role in the decision making of corporate boards. It has been observed in online networks and may help partly explain the spread of extremism in groups.

34
Q

Groupthink

A

In contrast to group polarization, which is a normal process in group dynamics, groupthink is more like a “disease” that can infect decision making in groups. Groupthink occurs when members of a cohesive group emphasize concurrence at the expense of critical thinking in arriving at a decision. As you might imagine, groupthink doesn’t produce very effective decision making. Indeed, groupthink can lead to major blunders that may look incomprehensible after the fact. Irving Janis first described groupthink in his effort to explain how former U.S. president John F. Kennedy and his advisors could have miscalculated so badly in deciding to invade Cuba at the Bay of Pigs in 1961. The attempted invasion failed miserably and, in retrospect, seemed remarkably ill-conceived.

35
Q

Illusory Correlation

A

People not only see what they expect to see, but also tend to overestimate how often they see it. Illusory correlation occurs when people estimate that they have encountered more confirmations of an association between social traits than they have actually seen. People also tend to underestimate the number of disconfirmations that they have encountered, as illustrated by statements like “I’ve never met an honest lawyer.”

36
Q

Implicit Attitudes

A

In recent years, theorists have begun to make a distinction between explicit and implicit attitudes. Explicit attitudes are attitudes that we hold consciously and can readily describe. For the most part, these overt attitudes are what social psychologists have always studied until fairly recently. Implicit attitudes are covert attitudes that are expressed in subtle automatic responses over which we have little conscious control. It was only in the mid-1990s that social psychologists started digging beneath the surface to explore the meaning and importance of implicit attitudes. People can have implicit attitudes about virtually anything. But implicit attitudes were discovered in research on prejudice and their role in various types of prejudice continues to be the main focus of current inquiry.

37
Q

Individualism

A

Decades of research have shown that cultural differences in individualism versus collectivism influence attributional tendencies as well as many other aspects of social behavior. Individualism involves putting personal goals ahead of group goals and defining one’s identity in terms of personal attributes rather than group memberships. In contrast, collectivism involves putting group goals ahead of personal goals and defining one’s identity in terms of the groups one belongs to (such as one’s family, tribe, work group, social class, and caste).

38
Q

Informational Influence

A

Why do people conform? Two key processes appear to contribute. Normative influence operates when people conform to social norms for fear of negative social consequences. In other words, people often conform or comply because they are afraid of being criticized or rejected. People are also likely to conform when they are uncertain how to behave. Informational influence operates when people look to others for guidance about how to behave in ambiguous situations. Thus, if you’re at a nice restaurant and don’t know which fork to use, you may watch others to see what they’re doing. In situations like this, using others as a source of information about appropriate behavior is a sensible strategy. Ultimately, informational influence is all about being right, whereas normative influence is all about being liked.

39
Q

Ingroup

A

What about the human tendency to automatically categorize others? Evolutionary theorists attribute this behavior to our distant ancestors’ need to quickly separate friend from foe. They assert that humans are programmed by evolution to immediately classify people as members of an ingroup—a group that one belongs to and identifies with, or as members of an outgroup—a group that one does not belong to or identify with. This crucial categorization is thought to structure subsequent perceptions.

40
Q

Internal Attributions

A

Internal attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to personal dispositions, traits, abilities, and feelings.

41
Q

Interpersonal Attraction

A

Interpersonal attraction refers to positive feelings toward another. Social psychologists use this term broadly to encompass a variety of experiences, including liking, friendship, admiration, lust, and love.

42
Q

Intimacy

A

The distinction between passionate and companionate love has been further refined by Robert Sternberg. He subdivides companionate love into intimacy and commitment. Intimacy refers to warmth, closeness, and sharing in a relationship. Commitment is an intent to maintain a relationship in spite of the difficulties and costs that may arise.

43
Q

Lowball Technique

A

The lowball technique is even more deceptive. The name for this technique derives from a common practice in automobile sales, in which a customer is offered a terrific bargain on a car. The bargain price gets the customer to commit to buying the car. Soon after this commitment is made, however, the dealer starts revealing some hidden costs. Typically, the customer learns that options assumed to be included in the original price are actually going to cost extra. Once they have committed to buying a car, most customers are unlikely to cancel the deal. Thus, the lowball technique involves getting someone to commit to an attractive proposition before its hidden costs are revealed.

44
Q

Matching Hypothesis

A

Although people prefer physically attractive partners in romantic relationships, they may consider their own level of attractiveness in pursuing dates. The matching hypothesis proposes that males and females of approximately equal physical attractiveness are likely to select each other as partners. The matching hypothesis is supported by evidence that dating and married couples tend to be similar in level of physical attractiveness. There is some debate, however, about whether people match up by mutual choice. Some theorists believe that individuals mostly pursue highly attractive partners and that their matching is the result of social forces beyond their control, such as rejection by more attractive others.

45
Q

Mere Exposure Effect

A

The mere exposure effect is the finding that repeated exposures to a stimulus promotes greater liking of the stimulus. In a groundbreaking study (Zajonc, 1968), participants were exposed to unfamiliar Turkish words 0, 1, 2, 5, 10, or 25 times. Subsequently, the subjects were asked to rate the degree to which they thought the words referred to something good or bad. The more subjects had been exposed to a specific word, the more favorably they rated it. Zajonc observed remarkably similar findings when participants rated the favorability of selected Chinese pictographs (the symbols used in Chinese writing) and when they rated the likability of people shown in yearbook photos. The mere exposure effect has been replicated with many types of stimuli.

46
Q

Message

A

The process of persuasion includes an examination of factors that affect persuasion, with research in the area emphasizing four basic elements: source, receiver, message, and channel. The source is the person who sends a communication, and the receiver is the person to whom the message is sent. So, if you watch a political news conference on TV, the politician is the source, and you and millions of other viewers are the receivers. The message is the information transmitted by the source, and the channel is the medium through which the message is sent. Although the research on communication channels is interesting, we’ll confine our discussion to source, message, and receiver variables, which are most applicable to persuasion.

47
Q

Normative Influence

A

Normative influence operates when people conform to social norms for fear of negative social consequences. In other words, people often conform or comply because they are afraid of being criticized or rejected. People are also likely to conform when they are uncertain how to behave.

48
Q

Obedience

A

Obedience is a form of compliance that occurs when people follow direct commands, usually from someone in a position of authority. To a surprising extent, when an authority figure says, “Jump!” many people simply ask, “How high?” For most people, willingness to obey someone in authority is the rule, not the exception.

49
Q

Outgroup

A

What about the human tendency to automatically categorize others? Evolutionary theorists attribute this behavior to our distant ancestors’ need to quickly separate friend from foe. They assert that humans are programmed by evolution to immediately classify people as members of an ingroup—a group that one belongs to and identifies with, or as members of an outgroup—a group that one does not belong to or identify with.

50
Q

Passionate Love

A

Passionate love is a complete absorption in another that includes tender sexual feelings and the agony and ecstasy of intense emotion.

51
Q

Person Perception

A

While we often gather many pieces of information about another person, our final impressions can often be dramatically affected by just one piece of information. In some of the classic research into impression formation, Solomon Asch demonstrated the importance that what he called central traits can have on the impressions we form of others. When you interact with people, you’re constantly engaged in person perception, the process of forming impressions of others. People show considerable ingenuity in piecing together clues about others’ characteristics. However, impressions are often inaccurate because of the many biases and fallacies that occur in person perception.

52
Q

Prejudice

A

Prejudice and discrimination are closely related, but not interchangeable concepts. Prejudice is a negative attitude held toward members of a group. Like other attitudes, prejudice can include three components: beliefs (“Indigenous males are mostly alcoholics”), emotions (“I despise”), and behavioral dispositions (“I wouldn’t hire a woman”). Racial and ethnic prejudice receives the lion’s share of publicity, but prejudice is not limited to ethnic groups. Women, gays, the aged, the disabled, the homeless, and the mentally ill are also targets of widespread prejudice. Thus, many people hold prejudicial attitudes toward one group or another, and many have been victims of prejudice.

53
Q

Receiver

A

The process of persuasion includes an examination of factors that affect persuasion, with research in the area emphasizing four basic elements: source, receiver, message, and channel. The source is the person who sends a communication, and the receiver is the person to whom the message is sent. So, if you watch a political news conference on TV, the politician is the source, and you and millions of other viewers are the receivers. The message is the information transmitted by the source, and the channel is the medium through which the message is sent. Although the research on communication channels is interesting, we’ll confine our discussion to source, message, and receiver variables, which are most applicable to persuasion.

54
Q

Reciprocity Norm

A

Most of us have been socialized to believe in the reciprocity norm—the rule that we should pay back in kind what we receive from others. Robert Cialdini has written extensively about how the reciprocity norm is used in social influence efforts. For example, groups seeking donations routinely send address labels, key rings, and other small gifts with their pleas. Salespeople using the reciprocity principle distribute free samples to prospective customers. When they return a few days later, most of the customers feel obligated to buy some of their products. The reciprocity rule is meant to promote fair exchanges in social interactions. However, when people manipulate the reciprocity norm, they usually give something of minimal value in the hopes of getting far more in return. Many Internet scams involve manipulations of reciprocity.

55
Q

Self-Serving Bias

A

The self-serving bias in attribution comes into play when people attempt to explain success and failure. The self-serving bias is the tendency to attribute one’s successes to personal factors and one’s failures to situational factors. In explaining failure, the usual actor-observer biases are apparent. But in explaining success, the usual actor-observer differences are reversed to some degree: actors prefer internal attributions so they can take credit for their triumphs. Interestingly, this bias grows stronger as time passes after an event, so that people tend to take progressively more credit for their successes and less blame for their failures. The self-serving bias is intended to bolster self-esteem and subjective well-being, and the evidence suggests that it is at least partially successful in this regard.

56
Q

Social Loafing

A

Social loafing is a reduction in effort by individuals when they work in groups as compared to when they work by themselves. To investigate social loafing, Latané and his colleagues measured the sound output produced by subjects who were asked to cheer or clap as loud as they could. So that they couldn’t see or hear other group members, the subjects were told that the study concerned the importance of sensory feedback and were asked to don blindfolds and put on headphones through which loud noise was played. This maneuver permitted a simple deception: Subjects were led to believe that they were either working alone or in a group of two or six, when in fact they were working alone and individual output was actually being measured.

57
Q

Social Neuroscience

A

Social psychology is the branch of psychology concerned with the way individuals’ thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by others.

58
Q

Social Roles

A

Social roles are widely shared expectations about how people in certain positions are supposed to behave. We have role expectations for salespeople, waiters, ministers, medical patients, students, bus drivers, tourists, flight attendants, and, of course, prison guards and prisoners.

59
Q

Social Schemas

A

Social schemas are organized clusters of ideas about categories of social events and people. People have social schemas for events such as dates, picnics, committee meetings, and family reunions, as well as for certain categories of people, such as “dumb jocks,” “social climbers,” “frat rats,” and “wimps”. Individuals depend on social schemas because the schemas help them to efficiently process and store the wealth of information that they take in about others in their interactions. Hence, people routinely place one another in categories, and these categories influence the process of person perception.

60
Q

Source

A

The process of persuasion includes an examination of factors that affect persuasion, with research in the area emphasizing four basic elements: source, receiver, message, and channel. The source is the person who sends a communication, and the receiver is the person to whom the message is sent. So, if you watch a political news conference on TV, the politician is the source, and you and millions of other viewers are the receivers. The message is the information transmitted by the source, and the channel is the medium through which the message is sent. Although the research on communication channels is interesting, we’ll confine our discussion to source, message, and receiver variables, which are most applicable to persuasion.

61
Q

Stereotypes

A

Stereotypes are special types of schemas that fall into the latter category. Stereotypes are widely held beliefs that people have certain characteristics because of their membership in a particular group.

62
Q

Describe how various aspects of physical appearance may influence our impressions of others.

A

Studies have shown that judgments of others’ personality are often swayed by their appearance, especially their physical attractiveness. One recent study showed that good-looking people command more of our attention than less attractive individuals do. While in many cases there are benefits to being perceived accurately, perhaps if you are attractive you are willing to forgo some of this accuracy; research suggests that people tend to ascribe desirable personality characteristics to those who are good-looking. Attractive people tend to be seen as more sociable, friendly, poised, warm, and well adjusted than those who are less attractive. One recent study, focusing on the Big Five personality traits, found that attractive women were viewed as more agreeable, extraverted, conscientious, open to experience, and emotionally stable (lower in neuroticism) than less attractive women. In reality, research findings suggest that little correlation exists between attractiveness and personality traits.
Karen Dion found that not only were the attractive targets ascribed all those positive characteristics, but they were also expected to have better lives, to be better spouses, and to be more successful in their chosen careers.Dion showed that linking the beautiful with the good starts early. In that study, three- to six-and-a-half-year-old children were shown pictures of attractive and unattractive kids, and were asked to make a series of judgments.The attractive kids were viewed more positively on a variety of dimensions. For example: Unattractive kids are scarier and hit you without a good reason, while attractive kids won’t hit you back even if you hit them first.
You might guess that physical attractiveness would influence perceptions of competence less than perceptions of personality, but the data suggest otherwise. A recent review of the relevant research found that people have a surprisingly strong tendency to view good-looking individuals as more competent than less attractive individuals. This bias literally pays off for good-looking people, as they tend to secure better jobs and earn higher salaries than less attractive individuals.

63
Q

Explain how schemas, stereotypes, and other factors contribute to selectivity in person perception.

A

Stereotypes and other schemas create biases in person perception that frequently lead to confirmation of people’s expectations about others. Social psychologist James Olson, if someone’s behavior is ambiguous, people are likely to interpret what they see in a way that’s consistent with their expectations. So, after dealing with a pushy female customer, a salesman who holds traditional gender stereotypes might characterize the woman as “emotional.” In contrast, he might characterize a male who exhibits the same pushy behavior as “aggressive.”
People not only see what they expect to see, but also tend to overestimate how often they see it. Illusory correlation occurs when people estimate that they have encountered more confirmations of an association between social traits than they have actually seen. People also tend to underestimate the number of disconfirmations that they have encountered, as illustrated by statements like “I’ve never met an honest lawyer.”
Memory processes can contribute to confirmatory biases in person perception in a variety of ways. Often, individuals selectively recall facts that fit with their schemas and stereotypes. Evidence for such a tendency was found in a study by Cohen. In this experiment, participants watched a videotape of a woman, described as either a waitress or a librarian, who engaged in a variety of activities, including listening to classical music, drinking beer, and watching TV. When asked to recall what the woman did during the filmed sequence, participants tended to remember activities consistent with their stereotypes of waitresses and librarians. For instance, subjects who thought the woman was a waitress tended to recall her drinking beer, while subjects who thought she was a librarian tended to recall her listening to classical music.

64
Q

Summarize the evolutionary perspective on bias in person perception.

A

Why is the process of person perception riddled with bias? Evolutionary psychologists like Simon Fraser University’s Dennis Krebs argue that some of the biases seen in social perception were adaptive in humans’ ancestral environment. For example, they argue that person perception is swayed by physical attractiveness because attractiveness was associated with reproductive potential in women and with health, vigor, and the accumulation of material resources in men.
What about the human tendency to automatically categorize others? Evolutionary theorists attribute this behavior to our distant ancestors’ need to quickly separate friend from foe. They assert that humans are programmed by evolution to immediately classify people as members of an ingroup—a group that one belongs to and identifies with, or as members of an outgroup—a group that one does not belong to or identify with. This crucial categorization is thought to structure subsequent perceptions. As Krebs and Denton put it, “It is as though the act of classifying others as ingroup or outgroup activates two quite different brain circuits”. Ingroup members tend to be viewed in a favorable light, whereas outgroup members tend to be viewed in terms of various negative stereotypes. According to Krebs and Denton, these negative stereotypes (“They are inferior; they are all alike; they will exploit us”) move outgroups out of our domain of empathy, so we feel justified in not liking them or in discriminating against them, or even in some circumstances dehumanizing them.
Evolutionary psychologists, then, ascribe much of the bias in person perception to cognitive mechanisms that have been shaped by natural selection

65
Q

What are attributions? Why and when do we make them?

A

Attributions are inferences that people draw about the causes of events, others’ behavior, and their own behavior. If you conclude that a friend turned down your invitation because she’s overworked, you have made an attribution about the cause of her behavior (and, implicitly, have rejected other possible explanations). If you conclude that you’re stuck at home with nothing to do because you failed to plan ahead, you’ve made an attribution about the cause of an event (being stuck at home). If you conclude that you failed to plan ahead because you’re a procrastinator, you’ve made an attribution about the cause of your own behavior. People make attributions mainly because they have a strong need to understand their experiences. They want to make sense out of their own behavior, others’ actions, and the events in their lives.

66
Q

Distinguish between internal and external attributions. Summarize Weiner’s theory of attribution.

A

Elaborating on Heider’s insight, various theorists have agreed that explanations of behavior and events can be categorized as internal or external attributions. Internal attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to personal dispositions, traits, abilities, and feelings. External attributions ascribe the causes of behavior to situational demands and environmental constraints. For example, if a friend’s business fails, you might attribute it to his or her lack of business acumen (an internal, personal factor) or to negative trends in the nation’s economic climate (an external, situational explanation). Parents who find out that their teenage son has just banged up the car may blame it on his carelessness (a personal disposition) or on slippery road conditions (a situational factor).
Some psychologists have sought to discover additional dimensions of attributional thinking besides the internal–external dimension. After studying the attributions that people make in explaining success and failure, Bernard Weiner concluded that people often focus on the stability of the causes underlying behavior. According to Weiner, the stable–unstable dimension in attribution cuts across the internal–external dimension, creating four types of attributions for success and failure.
Let’s apply Weiner’s model to a concrete event. Imagine that you’re contemplating why you failed to get a job that you wanted. You might attribute your setback to internal factors that are stable (lack of ability) or unstable (inadequate effort to put together an eye-catching résumé). Or you might attribute your setback to external factors that are stable (too much outstanding competition) or unstable (bad luck). If you got the job, your explanations for your success would fall into the same four categories: internal–stable (your excellent ability), internal–unstable (your hard work to assemble a superb résumé), external–stable (lack of top-flight competition), and external–unstable (good luck).
Weiner’s model can be used to understand complex issues in the real world. For example, when people analyze the causes of poverty, their explanations tend to fit neatly into the cells of Weiner’s model: internal-stable (laziness, lack of thrift); internal-unstable (financially draining illness); external-stable (discrimination, inadequate government programs for training); and external-unstable (bad luck, economic recession).

67
Q

Describe several types of attributional bias and cultural variations in attributional tendencies.

A

Attributions are only inferences. Your attributions may not be the correct explanations for events. Paradoxical as it may seem, people often arrive at inaccurate explanations even when they contemplate the causes of their own behavior. Attributions ultimately represent guesswork about the causes of events, and these guesses tend to be slanted in certain directions. Let’s look at the principal biases seen in attribution.
Actor–Observer Bias
Your view of your own behavior can be quite different from the view of someone else observing you. When an actor and an observer draw inferences about the causes of the actor’s behavior, they often make different attributions. A common form of bias seen in observers is the fundamental attribution error, which refers to observers’ bias in favor of internal attributions in explaining others’ behavior. Of course, in many instances, an internal attribution may not be an “error.” However, observers have a curious tendency to overestimate the likelihood that an actor’s behavior reflects personal qualities rather than situational factors. Why? One reason is that situational pressures may not be readily apparent to an observer. In addition, it seems as if attributing others’ behavior to their dispositions is a relatively effortless, almost automatic process, whereas explaining people’s behavior in terms of situational factors requires more thought and effort. Another factor favoring internal attributions is that many people feel that few situations are so coercive that they negate all freedom of choice.
To illustrate the gap that often exists between actors’ and observers’ attributions, imagine that you’re visiting your bank and you fly into a rage over a mistake made on your account. Observers who witness your rage are likely to make an internal attribution and infer that you are surly, temperamental, and quarrelsome. They may be right, but if asked, you’d probably attribute your rage to the frustrating situation. Perhaps you’re normally a calm, easy going person, but today you’ve been in line for 20 minutes, you just straightened out a similar error by the same bank last week, and you’re being treated rudely by the teller. Observers are often unaware of historical and situational considerations such as these, so they tend to make internal attributions for another’s behavior.
In contrast, the circumstances that have influenced an actor’s behavior tend to be more salient to the actor. Hence, actors are more likely than observers to locate the cause of their behavior in the situation. In the actor–observer bias, actors favor external attributions for their behavior, whereas observers are more likely to explain the same behavior with internal attributions.
Self-Serving Bias
The self-serving bias in attribution comes into play when people attempt to explain success and failure. The self-serving bias is the tendency to attribute one’s successes to personal factors and one’s failures to situational factors. In explaining failure, the usual actor-observer biases are apparent. But in explaining success, the usual actor-observer differences are reversed to some degree: actors prefer internal attributions so they can take credit for their triumphs. Interestingly, this bias grows stronger as time passes after an event, so that people tend to take progressively more credit for their successes and less blame for their failures. The self-serving bias is intended to bolster self-esteem and subjective well-being, and the evidence suggests that it is at least partially successful in this regard.
Defensive Attribution
The defensive attribution is the tendency to blame victims for their misfortunes, so that one feels less likely to be victimized in a similar way. The defensive attribution is relevant to situations in which you are attempting to explain calamities and setbacks that befall others. Here, an observer’s tendency to make internal attributions becomes even stronger than normal. Blaming a victim helps people maintain their belief in a just world, where they are unlikely to suffer a similar fate.
Individualism vs Collectivism
Decades of research have shown that cultural differences in individualism versus collectivism influence attributional tendencies as well as many other aspects of social behavior. Individualism involves putting personal goals ahead of group goals and defining one’s identity in terms of personal attributes rather than group memberships. In contrast, collectivism involves putting group goals ahead of personal goals and defining one’s identity in terms of the groups one belongs to (such as one’s family, tribe, work group, social class, and caste). In comparison with individualistic cultures, collectivist cultures place a higher priority on shared values and resources, cooperation, mutual interdependence, and concern for how one’s actions will affect other group members. Generally speaking, North American and Western European cultures tend to be individualistic, whereas Asian, African, and Latin American cultures tend to be collectivistic.
How does individualism versus collectivism relate to patterns of attribution? The evidence suggests that collectivist cultures may promote different attributional biases than individualistic cultures do. For example, although people from collectivist societies are not immune to the fundamental attribution error, they appear to be less susceptible to it than those from individualistic societies. Research also suggests that self-serving bias may be particularly prevalent in individualistic, Western societies, where an emphasis on competition and high self-esteem motivates people to try to impress others, as well as themselves. Like the fundamental attribution error, the self-serving bias is seen in collectivist cultures, but not as frequently as in Western societies.

68
Q

Summarize the evidence for three key factors in attraction.

A

It is often said that “beauty is only skin deep.” But the empirical evidence suggests that most people don’t behave in a manner that is consistent with this saying. Research shows that the key determinant of romantic attraction for both genders is the physical attractiveness of the other person. Many studies have demonstrated the singular prominence of physical attractiveness in the initial stage of dating and have shown that it continues to influence the course of commitment as relationships evolve. In the realm of romance, being physically attractive appears to be more important for females’ desirability. For example, in a study of college students, the correlation between romantic popularity (assessed by peer ratings) and physical attractiveness was higher for females (0.76) than for males (0.47).
Although people prefer physically attractive partners in romantic relationships, they may consider their own level of attractiveness in pursuing dates. The matching hypothesis proposes that males and females of approximately equal physical attractiveness are likely to select each other as partners. The matching hypothesis is supported by evidence that dating and married couples tend to be similar in level of physical attractiveness . There is some debate, however, about whether people match up by mutual choice. Some theorists believe that individuals mostly pursue highly attractive partners and that their matching is the result of social forces beyond their control, such as rejection by more attractive others.
According to the matching hypothesis, males and females who are similar in physical attractiveness are likely to be drawn together. This type of matching may also influence the formation of friendships.
Is it true that “birds of a feather flock together,” or do “opposites attract”? Research provides far more support for the former than the latter. One study found that people sit closer to others who are similar to them on simple physical traits, such as hair length, hair color, and whether they wear glasses. Married and dating couples tend to be similar in age, race, religion, social class, education, intelligence, physical attractiveness, and attitudes. The similarity principle operates in both friendships and romantic relationships regardless of sexual orientation. The most obvious explanation for these correlations is that similarity causes attraction, perhaps because we assume that similar others will like us. However, research also suggests that attraction can foster similarity because people who are close gradually modify their attitudes in ways that make them more congruent, a phenomenon called attitude alignment.
There is absolutely no empirical evidence to support this folklore. Research consistently shows that couples tend to be similar in intelligence, education, social status, ethnicity, physical attractiveness, and attitudes. Dissimilarity does not foster attraction.
In interpersonal attraction, reciprocity involves liking those who show that they like us. In general, research indicates that we tend to like those who show that they like us and that we tend to see others as liking us more if we like them. Thus, it appears that liking breeds liking, and loving promotes loving. Reciprocating attraction generally entails providing friends and intimate partners with positive feedback that results in a self-enhancement effect—in other words, you help them feel good about themselves.

69
Q

Describe the different categorizations of love. Describe cultural variations in romantic attraction.

A

Two early pioneers in research on love were Elaine Hatfield and Ellen Berscheid. They have proposed that romantic relationships are characterized by two kinds of love: passionate love and companionate love. Passionate love is a complete absorption in another that includes tender sexual feelings and the agony and ecstasy of intense emotion. Companionate love is warm, trusting, tolerant affection for another whose life is deeply intertwined with one’s own. Passionate and companionate love can co-exist. They don’t, however, necessarily go hand in hand. Initially, it was thought that passionate love peaks in intensity early in relationships and then declines significantly over time. However, more recent research suggests that in relationships that remain intact, the erosion of passionate love tends to be gradual and modest, with levels remaining fairly high in most couples.
Research demonstrates that passionate love is a powerful motivational force that produces profound changes in people’s thinking, emotion, and behavior. Interestingly, brain-imaging research indicates that when people think about someone they are passionately in love with, these thoughts light up the dopamine circuits in the brain that are known to be activated by cocaine and other addictive drugs. Perhaps that explains why passionate love sometimes resembles an addiction.
Research suggests that, as a general rule, companionate love is more strongly related to relationship satisfaction than passionate love. The distinction between passionate and companionate love has been further refined by Robert Sternberg. He subdivides companionate love into intimacy and commitment. Intimacy refers to warmth, closeness, and sharing in a relationship. Commitment is an intent to maintain a relationship in spite of the difficulties and costs that may arise.
In a groundbreaking analysis of love, Hazan and Shaver looked at similarities between adult love and attachment relationships in infancy. Early attachments vary in quality, and infants tend to fall into three groups. Most infants develop a secure attachment. However, some are very anxious when separated from their caregiver, a syndrome called anxious-ambivalent attachment. A third group of infants, characterized by avoidant attachment, never bond very well with their caregiver.
According to Hazan and Shaver, romantic love is an attachment process, and people’s intimate relationships in adulthood follow the same form as their attachments in infancy. In their theory, a person who had an anxious-ambivalent attachment in infancy will tend to have romantic relations marked by anxiety and ambivalence in adulthood. In other words, people relive their early bonding experiences with their parents in their romantic relationships in adulthood.
Hazan and Shaver’s initial survey study provided striking support for their theory. They found that adults’ love relationships could be sorted into groups that paralleled the three patterns of attachment seen in infants. Secure adults found it relatively easy to get close to others and described their love relations as trusting. Anxious-ambivalent adults reported a preoccupation with love, accompanied by expectations of rejection, and they described their love relations as volatile and marked by jealousy. Avoidant adults found it difficult to get close to others and described their love relations as lacking intimacy and trust. Research eventually showed that attachment patterns are reasonably stable over time and that people’s working models of attachment are carried forward from one relationship to the next. These findings supported the notion that individuals’ infant attachment experiences shape their intimate relations in adulthood.
Relatively little cross-cultural research has been conducted on the dynamics of close relationships. The limited evidence suggests both similarities and differences between cultures in romantic relationships. Psychologists such as the University of Toronto’s Karen and Ken Dion suggest that cultures vary considerably in terms of how they understand and conceptualize love and relationships. They suggest that some of this variability is attributable to differences in societal and psychological differences in individualism and collectivism.
For the most part, similarities have been seen when research has focused on what people look for in prospective mates—such as mutual attraction, kindness, and intelligence. Cultures vary, however, in their emphasis on love—especially passionate love—as a prerequisite for marriage. Passionate love as the basis for marriage is an 18th-century invention of individualistic Western culture. In contrast, marriages arranged by families and other go-betweens remain common in cultures high in collectivism, including India, Japan, China, and many Middle Eastern countries. In collectivist societies, people contemplating marriage tend to think in terms of “What will my parents and other people say?” rather than “What does my heart say?” Although romantic love is routinely seen in collectivist societies, subjects from those societies are less likely than subjects from cultures high in individualism to report that romantic love is important for marriage.
While mixed marriages are on the rise in Canada, sometimes the clash of cultural values can have tragic consequences. For example, Rajinder Singh Atwal of New Westminster, British Columbia, was convicted of second-degree murder in the July 2003 stabbing death of his 17-year-old-daughter, Amandeep, because she was in a relationship with Todd McIsaac, a boy of a different cultural group and religion. Atwal received an automatic life sntence, with no chance of parole for 16 years. While honor killings in Canada are limited, they do occur periodically. The Department of Justice estimates that there were a dozen such murders in Canada between 1999 and 2009.

70
Q

Describe the evolutionary perspective on attraction.

A

Evolutionary psychologists have a great deal to say about heterosexual attraction. For example, they assert that physical appearance is an influential determinant of attraction because certain aspects of good looks can be indicators of sound health, good genes, and high fertility, all of which can contribute to reproductive potential. Consistent with the evolutionary view, research has found that some standards of attractiveness are more consistent across cultures than previously believed. For example, facial symmetry seems to be a key element of attractiveness in highly diverse cultures. Facial symmetry is thought to be valued because a variety of environmental insults and developmental abnormalities are associated with physical asymmetries, which can serve as markers of relatively poor genes or health. Another facet of appearance that may transcend culture is women’s waist-to-hip ratio. Around the world, men seem to prefer women with a moderately low waist-to-hip ratio (in the vicinity of 0.70), which roughly corresponds to an “hourglass figure.” This appears to be a meaningful correlate of females’ reproductive potential, as it signals that a woman is healthy, young, and not pregnant.
The most thoroughly documented findings on the evolutionary bases of heterosexual attraction are those on gender differences in mating preferences, which appear to be consistent across highly varied cultures. In keeping with the notion that humans are programmed by evolution to behave in ways that enhance their reproductive fitness, evidence indicates that men generally are more interested than women in seeking youthfulness and physical attractiveness in their mates because these traits should be associated with greater reproductive potential. On the other hand, research shows that women place a greater premium on prospective mates’ ambition, social status, and financial potential because these traits should be associated with the ability to invest material resources in children.
These findings have been questioned in some studies of speed dating, which found that dating prospects’ physical attractiveness did not differentially predict males’ and females’ romantic interest. However, critics have noted that the speed dating situation may evoke short-term mating strategies, and it has long been known that when women are asked about what they prefer in a short-term partner (for casual sex), they value physical attractiveness just as much as men do. Critics have also wondered whether the speed dating studies included enough variation in social status and attractiveness to provide a sensitive test of the impact of these variables. A recent study that included more people at the low end of the spectrum in terms of attractiveness and social status (thus increasing the variation on these dimensions) yielded results consistent with evolutionary theory: men placed more emphasis on the attractiveness of dating prospects, whereas women emphasized social status more. Moreover, another study that focused on couples in their first four years of marriage found that partners’ physical attractiveness influenced the relationship satisfaction of husbands more than wives.
Evolutionary analyses also make some interesting predictions about how women’s menstrual cycles may influence their mating preferences and tactics. When women are in mid-cycle approaching ovulation—that is, when they are most fertile—their preferences shift to favor men who exhibit masculine facial and bodily features, attractiveness, and dominance. Women’s mating strategies also change when their fertility is at its peak, as they tend to wear more provocative clothing and they are more flirtatious in the presence of attractive men. Interestingly, although ovulation is far from obvious in human females, strippers earn 58 percent more tip money per night when they are in their most fertile period. Researchers aren’t sure whether male patrons are “detecting” the strippers’ heightened fertility or whether the ovulating dancers come on to the customers more because they are more sexually motivated.

71
Q

Describe the three components of attitudes and the relationship between attitudes and behavior.

A

Attitudes can include up to three components. The cognitive component of an attitude is made up of the beliefs people hold about the object of an attitude. The affective component consists of the emotional feelings stimulated by an object of thought. The behavioral component consists of predispositions to act in certain ways toward an attitude object.
Attitudes also vary along several crucial dimensions. These include their strength, accessibility, and ambivalence. Definitions of attitude strength differ. However, strong attitudes are generally viewed as ones that are firmly held (resistant to change), that are durable over time, and that have a powerful impact on behavior. The accessibility of an attitude refers to how often one thinks about it and how quickly it comes to mind. Highly accessible attitudes are quickly and readily available. Ambivalent attitudes are conflicted evaluations that include both positive and negative feelings about an object of thought. When ambivalence is high, an attitude tends to be more pliable in the face of persuasion.
How well do attitudes predict actual behavior? Research on attitudes has yielded a surprising answer to this question. In the early 1930s, when prejudice against Asians was common in North America, Richard LaPiere journeyed across the United States with a Chinese couple. He was more than a little surprised when they weren’t turned away from any of the restaurants they visited in their travels—184 restaurants in all. About six months after his trip, LaPiere surveyed the same restaurants and asked whether they would serve Chinese customers. Roughly half of the restaurants replied to the survey, and over 90 percent of them indicated that they would not seat Chinese patrons. Thus, LaPiere found that people who voice prejudicial attitudes may not behave in discriminatory ways. Since then, theorists have often asked: Why don’t attitudes predict behavior better?
Admittedly, LaPiere’s study had a fundamental flaw that you may already have detected. The person who seated LaPiere and his Chinese friends may not have been the same person who responded to the mail survey sent later. The connection between attitudes and behavior has been the focus of a great deal of research since LaPiere’s study.
Studies have repeatedly shown that attitudes are mediocre predictors of people’s behavior. When Wallace and colleagues reviewed 797 attitude-behavior studies, they found that the average correlation between attitudes and behavior was 0.41. That correlation is high enough to conclude that attitudes are a meaningful predictor of actual behavior, but they do not predict behavior nearly as well as most people assume.

72
Q

Summarize evidence on source factors, message factors, and receiver factors that influence the process of persuasion.

A

Occasional exceptions to the general rule are seen, but persuasion tends to be more successful when the source has high credibility. What gives a person credibility? Either expertise or trustworthiness. People try to convey their expertise by mentioning their degrees, their training, and their experience or by showing an impressive grasp of the issue at hand. Expertise is a plus, but trustworthiness can be even more important. Many people tend to accept messages from trustworthy sources with little scrutiny. Trustworthiness is undermined when a source appears to have something to gain. Likability also increases the effectiveness of a persuasive source. In addition, people respond better to sources who share similarity with them in ways that are relevant to the issue at hand.
The importance of source variables can be seen in advertising. Many companies spend a fortune to obtain an ideal spokesperson. Right now one of the most sought-after spokespersons in Canada is Sidney Crosby. Crosby is billed as one of the superstars of the NHL and our Canadian Olympic Men’s Hockey team.
If you were going to give a speech to a local community group advocating a reduction in taxes on corporations, you’d probably wrestle with a number of questions about how to structure your message. Should you look at both sides of the issue, or should you present just your side? Should you use all of the arguments at your disposal, or should you concentrate on the stronger arguments? Should you deliver a low-key, logical speech? Or should you try to strike fear into the hearts of your listeners? These questions are concerned with message factors in persuasion.
In general, two-sided arguments seem to be more effective than one-sided presentations. Just mentioning that an issue has two sides can increase your credibility with an audience. Fear appeals appear to work—if the message is successful in arousing fear. Research reveals that many messages intended to induce fear fail to do so. Fear appeals are most likely to work when your listeners think the dire consequences you describe as exceedingly unpleasant are fairly probable if they don’t take your advice and are avoidable if they do.
Frequent repetition of a message also seems to be an effective strategy, probably because of the mere exposure effect first described by Robert Zajonc. The mere exposure effect is the finding that repeated exposures to a stimulus promotes greater liking of the stimulus. In a groundbreaking study, participants were exposed to unfamiliar Turkish words 0, 1, 2, 5, 10, or 25 times. Subsequently, the subjects were asked to rate the degree to which they thought the words referred to something good or bad. The more subjects had been exposed to a specific word, the more favorably they rated it. Zajonc observed remarkably similar findings when participants rated the favorability of selected Chinese pictographs (the symbols used in Chinese writing) and when they rated the likability of people shown in yearbook photos. The mere exposure effect has been replicated with many types of stimuli.
What about the receiver of the persuasive message? Are some people easier to persuade than others? Undoubtedly, but researchers have not found any personality traits that are reliably associated with susceptibility to persuasion. Other factors, such as the forewarning a receiver gets about a persuasive effort and the receiver’s initial position on an issue, generally seem to be more influential than the receiver’s personality.
An old saying suggests that “to be forewarned is to be forearmed.” The value of forewarning applies to targets of persuasive efforts. When you shop for a new TV, you expect salespeople to work at persuading you, and to some extent this forewarning reduces the impact of their arguments. Considerations that stimulate counterarguing in the receiver tend to increase resistance to persuasion.
Furthermore, studies show that stronger attitudes are more resistant to change. Strong attitudes may be tougher to alter because they tend to be embedded in networks of beliefs and values that might also require change. Finally, resistance can promote resistance. That is, when people successfully resist persuasive efforts to change specific attitudes, they often become more certain about those attitudes.

73
Q

Explain how cognitive dissonance can account for the effects of counterattitudinal behavior and effort justification.

A

Leon Festinger’s dissonance theory assumes that inconsistency among attitudes propels people in the direction of attitude change. Dissonance theory had a profound impact on the directions taken by researchers in social psychology, and in other disciplines. It burst into prominence in 1959 when Festinger and J. Merrill Carlsmith published a famous study of counterattitudinal behavior. Let’s look at their findings and at how dissonance theory explains them.
Festinger and Carlsmith had male college students come to a laboratory, where they worked on excruciatingly dull tasks such as turning pegs repeatedly. When a subject’s hour was over, the experimenter confided that some participants’ motivation was being manipulated by telling them that the task was interesting and enjoyable before they started it. Then, after a moment’s hesitation, the experimenter asked if the subject could help him out of a jam. His usual helper was delayed and he needed someone to testify to the next “subject” (really an accomplice) that the experimental task was interesting. He offered to pay the subject if he would tell the person in the adjoining waiting room that the task was enjoyable and involving.
This entire scenario was enacted to coax participants into doing something that was inconsistent with their true feelings—that is, to engage in counterattitudinal behavior. Some participants received a token payment of $1 for their effort, while others received a more substantial payment of $20 (an amount equivalent to about $164 today, in light of inflation). Later, a second experimenter inquired about the subjects’ true feelings regarding the dull experimental task.
Who do you think rated the task more favorably—the subjects who were paid $1 or those who were paid $20? Both common sense and learning theory would predict that the subjects who received the greater reward ($20) should come to like the task more. In reality, however, the subjects who were paid $1 exhibited more favorable attitude change—just as Festinger and Carlsmith had predicted. Why? Dissonance theory provides an explanation.
According to Festinger, cognitive dissonance exists when related cognitions are inconsistent—that is, when they contradict each other. Cognitive dissonance is thought to create an unpleasant state of tension that motivates people to reduce their dissonance—usually by altering their cognitions. In the study by Festinger and Carlsmith, the subjects’ contradictory cognitions were “The task is boring” and “I told someone the task was enjoyable.” The subjects who were paid $20 for lying had an obvious reason for behaving inconsistently with their true attitudes, so these subjects experienced little dissonance. In contrast, the subjects paid $1 had no readily apparent justification for their lie and experienced high dissonance. To reduce it, they tended to persuade themselves that the task was more enjoyable than they had originally thought. Thus, dissonance theory sheds light on why people sometimes come to believe their own lies.
Cognitive dissonance is also at work when people turn attitudinal somersaults to justify efforts that haven’t panned out, a syndrome called effort justification. Aronson and Mills studied effort justification by putting college women through a “severe initiation” before they could qualify to participate in what promised to be an interesting discussion of sexuality. In the initiation, the women had to read obscene passages out loud to a male experimenter. After all that, the highly touted discussion of sexuality turned out to be a boring, taped lecture on reproduction in lower animals. Subjects in the severe initiation condition experienced highly dissonant cognitions (“I went through a lot to get here” and “This discussion is terrible”). How did they reduce their dissonance? Apparently, by changing their attitude about the discussion, since they rated it more favorably than subjects in two control conditions. Effort justification may be at work in many facets of everyday life. For example, people who wait in line for an hour or more to get into an exclusive restaurant often praise the restaurant afterward even if they have been served a mediocre meal.

74
Q

Relate learning theory and the elaboration likelihood model to attitude change.

A

The elaboration likelihood model of attitude change, originally proposed by Richard Petty and John Cacioppo, asserts that there are two basic “routes” to persuasion. The central route is taken when people carefully ponder the content and logic of persuasive messages. The peripheral route is taken when persuasion depends on non message factors, such as the attractiveness and credibility of the source, or on conditioned emotional responses. For example, a politician who campaigns by delivering carefully researched speeches that thoughtfully analyze complex issues is following the central route to persuasion. In contrast, a politician who depends on marching bands, flag-waving, celebrity endorsements, and emotional slogans is following the peripheral route.
Both routes can lead to persuasion. However, according to the elaboration likelihood model, the durability of attitude change depends on the extent to which people elaborate on (think about) the contents of persuasive communications. Studies suggest that the central route to persuasion leads to more enduring attitude change than the peripheral route. Research also suggests that attitudes changed through central processes predict behavior better than attitudes changed through peripheral processes.

75
Q

Describe Asch’s work on conformity.

A

In the 1950s, Solomon Asch devised a clever procedure that reduced ambiguity about whether subjects were conforming, allowing him to investigate the variables that govern conformity. Let’s re-create one of Asch’s classic experiments, which have become the most widely replicated studies in the history of social psychology. The subjects are male undergraduates recruited for a study of visual perception. A group of seven subjects is shown a large card with a vertical line on it and the subjects are then asked to indicate which of three lines on a second card matches the original “standard line” in length. All seven subjects are given a turn at the task, and they announce their choice to the group. The subject in the sixth chair doesn’t know it, but everyone else in the group is an accomplice of the experimenter, and they’re about to make him wonder whether he has taken leave of his senses.
The accomplices give accurate responses on the first two trials. On the third trial, line 2 clearly is the correct response, but the first five “subjects” all say that line 3 matches the standard line. The genuine subject is bewildered and can’t believe his ears. Over the course of the next 15 trials, the accomplices all give the same incorrect response on 11 of them. How does the real subject respond? The line judgments are easy and unambiguous. So, if the participant consistently agrees with the accomplices, he isn’t making honest mistakes—he’s conforming.
Averaging across all 50 participants, Asch found that the young men conformed on 37 percent of the trials. The subjects varied considerably in their tendency to conform, however. Of the 50 participants, 13 never caved into the group, while 14 conformed on more than half of the trials. One could argue that the results show that people confronting a unanimous majority generally tend to resist the pressure to conform. However, given how clear and easy the line judgments were, most social scientists viewed the findings as a dramatic demonstration of humans’ propensity to conform.
In subsequent studies, Asch found that group size and group unanimity are key determinants of conformity. To examine the impact of group size, Asch repeated his procedure with groups that included from 1 to 15 ccomplices. Little conformity was seen when a subject was pitted against just one person, but conformity increased rapidly as group size went from two to four, and then leveled off. Thus, Asch reasoned that as groups grow larger, conformity increases—up to a point, a conclusion that has been echoed by other researchers.

76
Q

Describe Milgram’s study on obedience to authority, and outline the ensuing controversy.

A

Stanley Milgram wanted to study this tendency to obey authority figures. Like many other people after World War II, he was troubled by how readily the citizens of Germany had followed the orders of dictator Adolf Hitler, even when the orders required morally repugnant actions, such as the slaughter of millions of Jews. Milgram, who had worked with Solomon Asch, set out to design a standard laboratory procedure for the study of obedience, much like Asch’s procedure for studying conformity. The clever experiment that Milgram devised became one of the most famous and controversial studies in the annals of psychology. It has been hailed as a “monumental contribution” to science and condemned as “dangerous, dehumanizing, and unethical research” .
Milgram’s participants were a diverse collection of 40 men from the local community. They were told that they would be participating in a study concerned with the effects of punishment on learning. When they arrived at the lab, they drew slips of paper from a hat to get their assignments. The drawing was rigged so that the subject always became the “teacher” and an experimental accomplice became the “learner.”
In the research, participants were induced to use shocks on learners in a learning experiment as feedback when the learners failed at a rigged task. While the learners were in fact confederates and the shocks were nonexistent, participants believed that they had delivered potentially harmful shocks to another person because they were told to do so by an authority figure, the experimenter. In the experiment, participants were induced to increase the shocks to what they believed would be dangerous levels as the learner continued to make mistakes in his task performance. Most participants went ahead and increased the shocks. Decades after the research was conducted, it still generates spirited debate.
After his initial demonstration, Milgram tried about 20 variations on his experimental procedure, looking for factors that influence participants’ obedience. In one variation, Milgram moved the study away from Yale’s campus to see if the prestige of the university was contributing to the subjects’ obedience. When the study was run in a seedy office building by the “Research Associates of Bridgeport,” only a small decrease in obedience was observed (48 percent of the subjects gave all of the shocks). Even when the learner was in the same room with the subjects, 40 percent of the participants administered the full series of shocks. As a whole, Milgram was surprised at how high subjects’ obedience remained as he changed various aspects of his experiment.
That said, there were some situational manipulations that reduced obedience appreciably. For example, if the authority figure was called away and the orders were given by an ordinary person (supposedly another participant), full obedience dropped to 20 percent. In another version of the study, Milgram borrowed a trick from Asch’s conformity experiments and set up teams of three teachers that included two more accomplices. When they drew lots, the real subject was always selected to run the shock apparatus in consultation with the other two “teachers.” When both accomplices accepted the experimenter’s orders to continue shocking the learner, the pressure increased obedience a bit. However, if an accomplice defied the experimenter and supported the subject’s objections, obedience declined dramatically (only 10 percent of the subjects gave all the shocks), just as conormity had dropped rapidly when dissent surfaced in Asch’s conformity studies. These findings are interesting in that they provide further support for Milgram’s thesis that situational factors exert great influence over behavior. If the situational pressures favoring obedience are decreased, obedience declines, as one would expect.

77
Q

Discuss cultural variations in conformity and obedience.

A

Are conformity and obedience unique to American or Western culture? By no means. Conformity and obedience experiments have been repeated in many countries including Canada, where they have yielded results roughly similar to those seen in the United States. The replications of Milgram’s obedience study have largely been limited to industrialized nations similar to the United States. Comparisons of the results of these studies must be made with caution because the composition of the samples and the experimental procedures have varied somewhat. But many of the studies have reported even higher obedience rates than those seen in Milgram’s American samples. For example, obedience rates of over 80 percent have been reported for samples from Italy, Germany, Austria, Spain, and the Netherlands. So, the surprisingly high level of obedience observed by Milgram does not appear to be peculiar to the United States.
The Asch experiment has been repeated in a more diverse range of societies than the Milgram experiment. Like many other cultural differences in social behavior, variations in conformity appear subject to cultural influences, including being related to the degree of individualism versus collectivism seen in a society. Various theorists have argued that collectivistic cultures, which emphasize respect for group norms, cooperation, and harmony, probably encourage more conformity than individualistic cultures and have a more positive view of conformity. As Matsumoto puts it, “To conform in American culture is to be weak or deficient somehow. But this is not true in other cultures. Many cultures foster more collective, group-oriented values, and concepts of conformity, obedience, and compliance enjoy much higher status.” Consistent with this analysis, studies have found higher levels of conformity in collectivistic cultures than in individualistic cultures.

78
Q

Discuss the nature of groups and the bystander effect.

A

When it comes to helping behavior, many studies have uncovered an apparent paradox called the bystander effect: people are less likely to provide needed help when they are in groups than when they are alone.
Evidence that your probability of getting help declines as group size increases was first described by John Darley and Bibb Latané, who were conducting research on the determinants of helping behavior. Their research was motivated by an infamous incident in New York City in 1963 in which a young woman, Kitty Genovese, was murdered. It was reported in the newspapers that many people witnessed the murder but did nothing to help her, not even bothering to call the police while the assault was occurring. Darley and Latané wanted to understand the inaction of the bystanders.
In the Darley and Latané study, students in individual cubicles connected by an intercom participated in discussion groups of three sizes. Early in the discussion, a student who was an experimental accomplice hesitantly mentioned that he was prone to seizures. Later in the discussion, the same accomplice faked a severe seizure and cried out for help. Although a majority of participants sought assistance for the student, the tendency to seek help declined with increasing group size.
Similar trends have been seen in many other experiments, in which subjects had opportunities to respond to apparent emergencies, including fires, asthma attacks, faintings, crashes, and flat tires, as well as less pressing needs to answer a door or to pick up objects dropped by a stranger. Pooling the results of early research on the bystander effect, Latané and Nida estimated that participants who are alone provide help 75 percent of the time, whereas participants in the presence of others provide help only 53 percent of the time.
What accounts for the bystander effect? A number of factors may be at work, but the most important appears o be the diffusion of responsibility that occurs in a group situation. If you’re by yourself when you encounter someone in need of help, the responsibility to provide help rests squarely on your shoulders. However, if other people are present, the responsibility is divided among you, and you may all say to yourselves, “Someone else will help.”

79
Q

Summarize the evidence regarding group productivity and group decision making.

A

Individuals’ productivity often does decline in larger groups. This fact is unfortunate, as many important tasks can be accomplished only in groups. Group productivity is crucial to committees, sports teams, firefighting crews, sororities, study groups, symphonies, and work teams of all kinds, from the morning crew in a little diner to the board of directors of a major company.
Two factors appear to contribute to reduced individual productivity in larger groups. One factor is reduced efficiency resulting from the loss of coordination among workers’ efforts. As you put more people on a yearbook staff, for instance, you’ll probably create more and more duplication of effort and increase how often group members end up working at cross-purposes.
The second factor contributing to low productivity in groups involves effort rather than efficiency. Social loafing is a reduction in effort by individuals when they work in groups as compared to when they work by themselves. To investigate social loafing, Latané and his colleagues measured the sound output produced by subjects who were asked to cheer or clap as loud as they could. So that they couldn’t see or hear other group members, the subjects were told that the study concerned the importance of sensory feedback and were asked to don blindfolds and put on headphones through which loud noise was played. This maneuver permitted a simple deception: Subjects were led to believe that they were either working alone or in a group of two or six, when in fact they were working alone and individual output was actually being measured.
When participants thought that they were working in larger groups, their individual output declined. Since lack of coordination could not affect individual output, the subjects’ decreased sound production had to be due to reduced effort. Latané and his colleagues also had the same subjects clap and shout in genuine groups of two and six and found an additional decrease in production that was attributed to loss of coordination.
The social-loafing effect has been replicated in numerous studies in which subjects have worked on a variety of tasks, including cheering, pumping air, swimming in a relay race, solving mazes, evaluating editorials, and brainstorming for new ideas. Social loafing and the bystander effect appear to share a common cause: diffusion of responsibility in groups. As group size increases, the responsibility for doing a job is divided among more people, and many group members ease up because their individual contribution is less recognizable. Thus, social loafing occurs in situations where individuals can “hide in the crowd”.
Social loafing, however, is not inevitable. For example, social loafing is less likely when individuals’ personal contributions to productivity are readily identifiable, and when group norms encourage productivity and personal involvement. And social loafing is reduced when people work in smaller and more cohesive groups. Cultural factors may also influence the likelihood of social loafing. Studies with subjects from Japan, China, and Taiwan suggest that social loafing may be less prevalent in collectivistic cultures, which place a high priority on meeting group goals and contributing to one’s ingroups.
Productivity is not the only issue that commonly concerns groups. When people join together in groups, they often have to make decisions about what the group will do and how it will use its resources. There is good evidence that decision making in groups may sometimes display or accentuate important biases when compared to individual decision making. These biases may be reflected in a variety of decision-making contexts, such as the common chore of making predictions regarding completion of one of the group’s tasks. For example, research by Roger Buehler and his colleagues has shown that the tendency of individuals to make optimistic predictions regarding how long it will take to complete a task is accentuated as a result of group discussion.
Evaluating decision making is often more complicated than evaluating productivity. In many cases, the “right” decision may not be readily apparent. Who can say whether your study group ordered the right pizza or whether Parliament passed the right bills? Nonetheless, social psychologists have discovered some interesting tendencies in group decision making.

80
Q

Explain how Chapter 13 highlights three of the textbook’s unifying themes.

A

Our discussion of social psychology has provided a final embellishment of three of our seven unifying themes. One of these is the value of psychology’s commitment to empiricism—that is, its reliance on systematic observation through research to arrive at conclusions. The second theme that stands out is the importance of cultural factors in shaping behavior, and the third is the extent to which people’s experience of the world is highly subjective.
It’s easy to question the need to do scientific research on social behavior, because studies in social psychology often seem to verify common sense. While most people wouldn’t presume to devise their own theory of color vision, question the significance of REM sleep, or quibble about the principal causes of schizophrenia, everyone has beliefs about the nature of love, how to persuade others, and people’s willingness to help in times of need. So, when studies demonstrate that credibility enhances persuasion, or that good looks facilitate attraction, it’s tempting to conclude that social psychologists go to great lengths to document the obvious, and some critics say, “Why bother?”
You saw why in this chapter. Research in social psychology has repeatedly shown that the predictions of logic and common sense are often wrong. Consider just a few examples. Even psychiatric experts failed to predict the remarkable obedience to authority uncovered in Milgram’s research. The bystander effect in helping behavior violates mathematical logic. Dissonance research has shown that after a severe initiation, the bigger the letdown, the more favorable people’s feelings are. These principles defy common sense. Thus, research on social behavior provides dramatic illustrations of why psychologists put their faith in empiricism.
Our coverage of social psychology also demonstrated once again that, cross-culturally, behavior is characterized by both variance and invariance. Thus, we saw substantial cultural differences in patterns of attribution, the role of love in mating relationships, attitudes about conformity, the tendency to obey authority figures, and the likelihood of social loafing. Although basic social phenomena such as stereotyping, attraction, obedience, and conformity probably occur all over the world, cross-cultural studies of social behavior show that research findings based on North American samples may not generalize precisely to other cultures.
Research in social psychology is also uniquely well suited for making the point that people’s view of the world is highly personal and subjective. In this chapter, we saw how physical appearance can color perception of a person’s ability or personality, how social schemas can lead people to see what they expect to see in their interactions with others, how pressure to conform can make people begin to doubt their senses, and how groupthink can lead group members down a perilous path of shared illusions.

81
Q

Describe the difference between prejudice and discrimination. Summarize the roles that stereotyping and biases in attribution play in helping to perpetuate prejudice.

A

Prejudice and discrimination are closely related, but not interchangeable concepts. Prejudice is a negative attitude held toward members of a group. Like other attitudes, prejudice can include three components: beliefs (“Indigenous males are mostly alcoholics”), emotions (“I despise”), and behavioral dispositions (“I wouldn’t hire a woman”). Racial and ethnic prejudice receives the lion’s share of publicity, but prejudice is not limited to ethnic groups. Women, gays, the aged, the disabled, the homeless, and the mentally ill are also targets of widespread prejudice. Thus, many people hold prejudicial attitudes toward one group or another, and many have been victims of prejudice.Prejudice may lead to discrimination, which involves behaving differently, usually unfairly, toward the members of a group. Prejudice and discrimination tend to go hand in hand, but attitudes and behavior do not necessarily correspond. In our discussion, we’ll concentrate primarily on the attitude of prejudice.
There is no doubt that stereotypes play a large role in prejudice. That’s not to say that stereotypes are inevitably negative. Although it’s an overgeneralization, it’s hardly insulting to assert that Canadians are polite, that Americans are ambitious, or that the Japanese are industrious. Unfortunately, many people do subscribe to derogatory stereotypes of various ethnic groups.
Although studies suggest that negative racial stereotypes have diminished over the last 50 years, they’re not a thing of the past. According to a variety of investigators, such as York University’s Kerry Kawakami and her colleagues, modern racism has merely become more subtle. Many people carefully avoid overt expressions of prejudicial attitudes but covertly continue to harbor negative views of racial minorities.
Research indicates that stereotypes are so pervasive and insidious they are often activated automatically, even in people who truly renounce prejudice. Thus, a man who rejects prejudice against homosexuals may still feel uncomfortable sitting next to a gay male on a bus, even though he regards his reaction as inappropriate.
Stereotypes persist because the subjectivity of person perception makes it likely that people will see what they expect to see when they actually come into contact with groups that they view with prejudice. For example, Duncan had white subjects watch and evaluate interaction on a TV monitor that was supposedly live (actually it was a videotape) and varied the race of a person who gets into an argument and gives another person a slight shove. The shove was coded as “violent behavior” by 73 percent of the subjects when the actor was black but by only 13 percent of the subjects when the actor was white. As we’ve noted before, people’s perceptions are highly subjective. Because of stereotypes, even “violence” may lie in the eye of the beholder.
Attribution processes can also help perpetuate stereotypes and prejudice. Research taking its cue from Weiner’s model of attribution has shown that people often make biased attributions for success and failure. For example, men and women don’t get equal credit for their successes. Observers often discount a woman’s success by attributing it to good luck, sheer effort, or the ease of the task (except on traditional feminine tasks). In comparison, a man’s success is more likely to be attributed to his outstanding ability. For example, one recent study found that when a man and woman collaborate on a stereotypically “male” task, both male and female observers downplay the woman’s contribution. These biased patterns of attribution help sustain the stereotype that men are more competent than women.
Recall that the fundamental attribution error is a bias toward explaining events by pointing to the personal characteristics of the actors as causes (internal attributions). Research suggests that people are particularly likely to make this error when evaluating targets of prejudice. Thus, when people take note of ethnic neighborhoods dominated by crime and poverty, the personal qualities of the residents are blamed for these problems, while other explanations emphasizing situational factors (job discrimination, poor police service, and so on) are downplayed or ignored. The old saying “They should be able to pull themselves up by their bootstraps” is a blanket dismissal of how situational factors may make it especially difficult for minorities to achieve upward mobility.
If prejudice is an attitude, where does it come from? Many prejudices appear to be handed down as a legacy from parents. Frances Aboud shows that prejudicial attitudes can be found in children as young as ages four or five. This transmission of prejudice across generations presumably depends to some extent on observational learning. For example, if a young boy hears his father ridicule homosexuals, the boy’s exposure to his father’s attitude is likely to affect his own attitude about gays. If the young boy then goes to school and makes disparaging remarks about gays that are reinforced by approval from peers, his prejudice will be strengthened through operant conditioning. Although parents clearly are important, as children grow older, their peer groups may become more influential than parents and other authority figures. Like parents, peers can foster either prejudice or egalitarian atttudes, depending on the views they endorse. Of course, prejudicial attitudes are not acquired only through direct experience. Stereotypic portrayals of various groups in the media can also foster prejudicial attitudes.

82
Q

Summarize the research findings on analyzing credibility and social influence tactics.

A

The salesperson at your local health food store swears that a specific herb combination improves memory and helps people stay healthy. A popular singer touts a psychic hotline, where the operators can “really help” with the important questions in life. Speakers at a “historical society” meeting claim that the Holocaust never happened. These are just a few real-life examples of how people are always attempting to persuade the public to believe something. In these examples, the “something” people are expected to believe runs counter to the conventional or scientific view, but who is to say who is right? People are entitled to their own opinions, but that does not mean that all opinions are equally valid. Some opinions are just plain wrong, and others are highly dubious. Every person is not equally believable. In deciding what to believe, it is important to carefully examine the evidence presented and the logic of the argument that supports the conclusion. In deciding what to believe, you also need to decide whom to believe, a task that requires assessing the credibility of the source of the information.
Does the source have a vested interest in the issue at hand? If the source is likely to benefit in some way from convincing you of something, you need to take a skeptical attitude. In the examples just mentioned, it is easy to see how the salesclerk and the popular singer will benefit if you buy the products they are selling, but what about the so-called historical society? How would members benefit by convincing large numbers of people that the Holocaust never happened? Like the salesclerk and the singer, they are also selling something—in this case, a particular view of history that they hope will influence future events in certain ways. Someone does not have to have a financial gain at stake to have a vested interest in an issue. Of course, the fact that these sources have a vested interest does not necessarily mean that the information they are providing is false or that their arguments are invalid. But a source’s credibility needs to be evaluated with extra caution when the person or group has something to gain.
What are the source’s credentials? Does the person have any special training, an advanced degree, or any other basis for claiming special knowledge about the topic? The usual training for a salesclerk or a singer does not include how to assess research results in medical journals or claims of psychic powers. The Holocaust deniers are more difficult to evaluate. Some of them have studied history and written books on the topic, but the books are mostly self-published and few of these “experts” hold positions at reputable universities where scholars are subject to peer evaluation. That’s not to say that legitimate credentials ensure a source’s credibility. A number of popular diets that are widely regarded by nutritional experts as worthless, if not hazardous, were created and marketed by genuine physicians. Of course, these physicians have a vested interest in the diets, as they have made millions of dollars from them.
Is the information grossly inconsistent with the conventional view on the issue? Just being different from the mainstream view certainly does not make a conclusion wrong. But claims that vary radically from most other information on a subject should raise a red flag that leads to careful scrutiny. Bear in mind that charlatans and hucksters are often successful because they typically try to persuade people to believe things that they want to believe. Wouldn’t it be great if we could effortlessly enhance our memory, foretell the future, eat all we want and still lose weight, and earn hundreds of dollars per hour working at home? And wouldn’t it be nice if the Holocaust had never happened? It pays to be wary of wishful thinking.
What was the method of analysis used in reaching the conclusion? The purveyors of miracle cures and psychic advice inevitably rely on anecdotal evidence. But you have already learned about the perils and unreliability of anecdotal evidence. One method frequently used by charlatans is to undermine the credibility of conventional information by focusing on trivial inconsistencies. This is one of the many strategies used by the people who argue that the Holocaust never occurred. They question the credibility of thousands of historical documents, photographs, and artifacts, and the testimony of countless people, by highlighting small inconsistencies among historical records relating to trivial matters, such as the number of people transported to a concentration camp in a specific week, or the number of bodies that could be disposed of in a single day. Some inconsistencies are exactly what one should expect based on piecing together multiple accounts from sources working with different portions of incomplete information. But the strategy of focusing on trivial inconsistencies is a standard method for raising doubts about credible information.