ATTACHMENT Flashcards
(96 cards)
Attachment is
a strong, emotional, and reciprocal bond between two people, especially an infant and caregiver.
It endures over time.
CAREGIVER-INFANT INTERACTIONS
From the start babies have meaningful social interactions with their carers. Psychologists believe that these interactions have
important functions for the child’s social development.
In particular, good quality and early social interactions are associated with the successful development of attachments between babies and their caregivers.
ALERT PHASES:
babies have periodic ‘alert phases’ in which they signal (e.g. making eye contact) that they are ready for a spell of interaction. Research shows that mothers typically pick up on and respond to their baby’s alertness around two-thirds of the time although this varies according to the skill of the mother and external factors such as stress.
RECIPROCITY
An interaction is said to show reciprocity when
each person responds to the other and elicits a response from them a caregiver might respond to his baby’s smile by saying something and then this in turn elicits a response from his baby. This kind of reciprocal interaction is also sometimes called ‘turn-taking’.
ACTIVE INVOLVEMENT:
babies as well as caregivers actually take quite an active role. Both caregiver and baby can initiate interactions and they appear to take turns in doing so.
INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY
Two people are said to be ‘synchronised’ when
they carry out the same action simultaneously. It takes place when caregiver and baby interact in such a way that their actions and emotions mirror the other.
SYNCHRONY STUDY: Meltzoff and Moore observed the beginnings of interactional synchrony in babies.
PROCEDURE
An adult displayed one of three facial expressions or one of three distinctive gestures. The baby’s response was filmed and labelled by independent observers.
Isabella et al. observed
and found
30 mothers and babies together and assessed the degree of synchrony. The researchers also assessed the quality of mother-baby attachment. They found that high levels of synchrony were associated with better quality mother-baby attachment.
AO3: limitation of INFANT-CAREGIVER INTERACTIONS
may not be useful
A further limitation is that simply observing a behaviour does not tell us its developmental importance.
Feldman points out that ideas like synchrony and reciprocity simply give names to patterns of observable caregiver and baby behaviours. These are robust phenomena in the sense that they can be reliably observed, but they still may not be particularly useful in understanding child development as it does not tell us the purpose of these behaviours.
This means that we cannot be certain from observational research alone that reciprocity and synchrony are important for a child’s development.
Meltzoff and Moore
FINDINGS
Babies’ expression and gestures were more likely to mirror those of the adults more than chance would predict was a significant association.
Importance for attachment It is believed that interactional synchrony is important for the development of caregiver-infant attachment.
AO3: strength of INFANT-CAREGIVER INTERACTIONS
high control - good validity and reliability
One strength of the research on this topic is that caregiver-infant interactions are usually filmed in a laboratory.
This means that other activity, that might distract a baby, can be controlled. Also, using films means that observations can be recorded and analysed later. Therefore, it is unlikely that researchers will miss seeing key behaviours. Furthermore, having filmed interactions means that more than one observer can record data and establish the inter-rater reliability of observations. Finally, babies don’t know they are being observed, so their behaviour does not change in response to observation (demand characteristics is generally the main problem for overt observations).
Therefore, the data collected in such research should have good reliability and validity.
AO3: limitation of INFANT-CAREGIVER INTERACTIONS
hard to interpret baby’s behaviour
One limitation of research into caregiver-infant interaction is that it is hard to interpret a baby’s behaviour.
Young babies lack co-ordination and much of their bodies are almost immobile. The movements being observed are just small hand movements or subtle changes in expression. It is difficult to be sure, for example, whether a baby is smiling or just passing wind. It is also difficult to determine what is taking place from the baby’s perspective. For example, we cannot know whether a movement such as a hand twitch is random or triggered by something the caregiver has done.
This means we cannot be certain that the behaviours seen in caregiver-infant interactions have a special meaning.
STAGES OF ATTACHMENT
Schaffer and Emerson studied the attachment behaviours of babies.
PROCEDURE
The study involved 60, all were from Glasgow and the majority were from skilled working-class families.
Researchers visited babies and mothers in their own homes every month for the first year and again at 18 months.
The researchers asked the mothers questions about the kind of protest their babies showed in seven everyday separations, e.g. adult leaving the room.
This was designed to measure the babies’ attachment.
The researchers also assessed stranger anxiety - the babies anxiety response to unfamiliar people.
Schaffer and Emerson
Findings
65% mothers primary attachment, 27% mother-father joint primary attachment, 3% father primary attachment.
Schaffer and Emerson identified four distinct stages in the development of infant attachment behaviour.
They proposed that there were four identifiable stages of attachment, a sequence which is observed in all babies.
STAGE 1: ASOCIAL STAGE
first few weeks
behaviour towards humans and inanimate objects is fairly similar.
However, it isn’t entirely asocial because even at this stage babies show signs that they prefer to be with other people.
Babies also tend to show a preference for the company of familiar people and are more easily comforted by them.
STAGE 2: INDISCRIMINATE ATTACHMENT
From 2 to 7 months
babies start to display more obvious and observable social behaviours.
They now show a clear preference for being with other humans rather than inanimate objects.
They also recognise and prefer the company of familiar people.
However, at this stage babies usually accept cuddles and comfort from any person - hence the term ‘indiscriminate.
They do not usually show separation anxiety when caregivers leave their presence or stranger anxiety in the presence of unfamiliar people.
STAGE 3: SPECIFIC ATTACHMENT
From around 7 months
The majority of babies start to display the classic signs of attachment towards one particular person.
These signs include anxiety directed towards strangers (stranger anxiety), especially when their attachment figure is absent, and anxiety when separated from their attachment figure (separation anxiety).
This person with whom the attachment is formed is called the primary attachment figure.
This person is not necessarily the individual the child spends most time with but the one who offers the most interaction and responds to the baby’s ‘signals’ with the most skill.
This is the baby’s mother in 65% of cases.
STAGE 4: MULTIPLE ATTACHMENTS
Shortly after babies start to show attachment behaviour (e.g. stranger anxiety and separation anxiety) towards one person they usually extend this behaviour to multiple attachments with other people with whom they regularly spend time. These relationships are called secondary attachments. Schaffer and Emerson observed that 29% of the children formed secondary attachments within a month of forming a primary (specific) attachment. By the age of one year the majority of babies had developed multiple attachments.
AO3: strength of STAGES OF ATTACHMENT
good external validity
One strength of Schaffer and Emerson’s research is that it has good external validity.
Most of the observations were made by parents during ordinary activities and reported to the researchers. The alternative would have been to have researchers present to record observations. This might have distracted the babies or made them feel more anxious.
This means it is highly likely that the participants behaved naturally while being observed.
AO3: strength of STAGES OF ATTACHMENT
practical application - daycare use
Another strength of Schaffer and Emerson’s stages is that they have practical application in day care where babies are cared for outside of their home (by a non-family adult).
In the asocial and indiscriminate attachment stages day care is likely to be straightforward as babies can be comforted by any skilled adult. However, Schaffer and Emerson’s research tells us that day care, especially starting day care with an unfamiliar adult, may be problematic during the specific attachment stage.
This means that parents’ use of day care can be planned using Schaffer and Emerson’s stages
AO3: limitation of STAGES OF ATTACHMENT
low validity - hard to interpret behaviour
One limitation of Schaffer and Emerson’s stages is the validity of the measures they used to assess attachment in the asocial stage.
Young babies have poor co-ordination and are fairly immobile. If babies less than two months old felt anxiety in everyday situations they might have displayed this in quite subtle, hard-to-observe ways. This made it difficult for mothers to observe and report back to researchers on signs of anxiety and attachment in this age group.
This means that the babies may actually be quite social but, because of flawed methods, they appear to be asocial.
AO3: limitation of STAGES OF ATTACHMENT
issues of mothers being main observers
On the other hand, there are issues with asking the mothers to be the ‘observers’.
They were unlikely to be objective observers. They might have been biased in terms of what they noticed and what they reported, for example they might not have noticed when their baby was showing signs of anxiety or they may have misremembered it.
This means that even if babies behaved naturally their behaviour may not have been accurately recorded.
ATTACHMENT TO FATHERS
Perhaps the most basic question about the role of fathers is whether babies actually attach to them and, if so, when.
Available evidence suggests that fathers are much less likely to become babies’ first attachment figure compared to mothers.
Schaffer and Emerson they found that the majority of babies first became attached to
their mother at around 7 months.
In only 3% of cases the father was the first sole object of attachment.
In 27% of cases the father was the joint first object of attachment with the mother.
However, it appears that most fathers go on to become important attachment figures. 75% of the babies studied by Schaffer and Emerson formed an attachment with their father by the age of 18 months. This was determined by
the fact that the babies protested when their father walked away - a sign of attachment.