Early childhood Flashcards
(64 cards)
Describe the physical growth and change that takes place during early childhood.
From age 3 to 5, the typical child grows about 7 cm per year and gains about 2 kg.
Three-year-old boys at the 50th percentile are 96 cm tall and weigh 14.5 kg, and at age 5 are 110 cm and 18.5 kg. Three-year-old girls at the 50th percentile are 96 cm tall and weigh 14 kg, and at age 5 are 108 cm and 18 kg.
Stunting
A term referring to children short for their age.
Approximately 149 million children, or 22%, worldwide were stunted in 2020.
Teeth
By three, most children have the full set of 20 primary/baby teeth. They will be replaced with 32 permanent teeth through childhood, beginning at about age six. Process last until about 14.
Describe the changes in brain development that take place during early childhood
At age 3, the brain is about 70% of its adult weight, and at age 6, about 90%.
The frontal lobes grow faster than the rest of the cerebral cortex during early childhood. Growth in the frontal lobes underlies the advances in emotional regulation, foresight and planned behaviour that take place during the preschool years. Throughout the cerebral cortex, growth from age 3 to 15 takes place in spurts within the different lobes, followed by periods of vigorous synaptic pruning.
During early childhood, the number of neurons continues the decline that began in toddlerhood via synaptic pruning. The increase in brain size and weight during early childhood is due to an increase in dendritic connections between neurons and to myelination.
Four parts of the brain especially notable for their myelination during early childhood.
The corpus callosum
Cerebellum
Reticular formation
Hippocampus
Infantile amnesia
Inability to remember anything before age 2.
While we retain language, habits and general information, our memory for events and things that happened to us is not well retained. This type of memory for specific personal events is called autobiographical memory.
One theory proposes that autobiographical memory before age 2 is limited because the awareness of self becomes stable at about 2 years of age and serves as a new organiser around which events can be encoded, stored and retrieved in memory as personal. Another perspective proposes that the encoding of memories is promoted by language development because language allows us to tell ourselves a narrative of events and experiences; consequently, most autobiographical memory is encoded only after language development accelerates at age 2. Memory researchers have proposed that the answer lies in the development of the hippocampus. Specifically, the hippocampus is immature at birth and adds neurons at a high rate in the early years of development. The addition of so many new neurons may interfere with the existing memory circuits so that long-term memories cannot be formed until the production of neurons in the hippocampus declines in early childhood, as it becomes more fully developed.
Nutrition and malnutrition
Appetites vary a lot from day to day in early childhood, and the 5-year-old who barely touched dinner one night may eat nearly as much as Mum and Dad the next night.
About 70% of Australian children do eat the required serves of fruit, but only 3% eat the recommended serves of vegetables.
Because young children in developed countries often eat too much of unhealthy foods and too little of healthy foods, many of them have specific nutritional deficiencies despite living in cultures where food is abundant.
A five-year-old in a developed country is most likely to have a deficiency in calcium.
Anaemia
Iron deficiency, known as anaemia, is experienced by the majority of children under age 5 in developing countries. Anaemia causes fatigue, irritability and difficulty sustaining attention, which in turn lead to problems in cognitive and social development.
Illness and disease
From 1980 to 2018, the number of deaths worldwide of children under age 5 declined from 14 million to under 5.5 million, even though the world’s population more than doubled during that time.
The decline is due to a variety of factors, especially improved food production in developing countries and increased prevalence of childhood vaccinations.
In developed countries minor illnesses are common in early childhood, with most children experiencing 7–10 per year,
Injuries
Young children have high activity levels and their motor development is advanced enough for them to be able to run, jump and climb, but their cognitive development is not yet advanced enough for them to anticipate situations that might be dangerous. This combination leads to high rates of injuries in early childhood and these injuries are the most common reason for death and hospitalisation between 5 and 14 years.
Rates of unintentional injury among 1- to 14-year-olds in South Africa are 5 times higher than in developed countries; in Vietnam, rates are 4 times higher, and in China 3 times higher. Disease still accounts for 95% of deaths.
Gross and fine motor skills
Refine and develop skills learnt in toddlerhood.
Gender differences in gross motor development appear in early childhood, with boys generally becoming better at skills emphasising strength or size and girls becoming better at body-coordination skills.
Their growing fine motor abilities allow children to learn to do many things their parents had been doing for them, such as putting on a coat or shoes, and brushing their teeth.
Children in some cultures also learn to use chopsticks around 4.5 years old.
Describe the development of handedness and identify the consequences and cultural views of left-handedness.
Once children begin drawing or writing in early childhood, they show a clear preference for using their right or left hand, but handedness appears long before early childhood. In fact, even prenatally, fetuses show a definite preference for sucking the thumb of their right or left hand, with 90% preferring the right thumb.
Identical twins are more likely than ordinary siblings to differ in handedness, even though identical twins share 100% of their genotype and other siblings only about 50%. This appears to be due to the fact that twins usually lie in opposite ways within the uterus, whereas most singletons lie towards the left. Lying towards one side allows for greater movement and hence greater development of the hand on the other side, so most twins end up with one being right-handed and one being left-handed, while most singletons end up right-handed.
Left-handedness in culture
In addition, culture is also a big part of the picture. Historically, many cultures have viewed left-handedness as dangerous and evil, and have suppressed its development in children.
In Western languages, the word sinister is derived from a Latin word meaning ‘on the left’, and many paintings in Western art depict the devil as left-handed.
In many Asian and Middle Eastern cultures, only the left hand is supposed to be used for wiping up after defecation, and all other activities are supposed to be done mainly with the right hand.
In China, using the left hand is suppressed in many families from childhood onwards, and the prevalence of left-handedness is as low as 1%, far lower than the 10% figure in cultures where left-handedness is tolerated.
Left-handedness biologically
Left-handed infants are more likely to be born prematurely or to experience an unusually difficult birth, and there is evidence that brain damage prenatally or during birth can contribute to left-handedness. In early and middle childhood, left-handers are more likely to have problems learning to read and to have other verbal learning disabilities. This may have something to do with the fact that about one-quarter of left-handers process language in both hemispheres rather than primarily in the left hemisphere.
Left-handed children are more likely to show exceptional verbal and maths abilities. Left-handers are especially likely to have strong visual–spatial abilities, and consequently they are more likely than right-handers to become architects or artists. The majority of left-handers are in the normal range in their cognitive development, and show neither unusual problems nor unusual gifts. Hence, the widespread cultural prejudice against left-handers remains mysterious.
Explain the features of Piaget’s preoperational stage of cognitive development.
2 - 7 years old.
Piaget specified a number of areas of preoperational cognitive mistakes that are characteristic of early childhood, including conservation, egocentrism and classification.
During the latter part of toddlerhood, and especially in early childhood, that we become truly representational thinkers. Language requires the ability to represent the world symbolically, through words, and this is when language skills develop most dramatically. Once we can represent the world through language, we are freed from our momentary sensorimotor experience. With language, we can represent not only the present, but also the past and the future, not only the world as we see it before us, but also the world as we previously experienced it and the world as it will be.
Conservation
According to Piaget, children in early childhood lack the ability to understand conservation, the principle that the amount of a physical substance remains the same even if its physical appearance changes.
Centration
Young children’s thinking is centred, or focused, on one noticeable aspect of a cognitive problem to the exclusion of other important aspects.
Reversibility
Young children lack reversibility, the ability to reverse an action mentally.
Egocentrism
Another cognitive limitation of the preoperational stage, in Piaget’s view, is egocentrism, the inability to distinguish between your own perspective and another person’s perspective.
Animism
One aspect of egocentrism is animism, the tendency to attribute human thoughts and feelings to inanimate objects and forces.
Classification
Preoperational children also lack the capacity for classification, according to Piaget, meaning that they have difficulty understanding that objects can be simultaneously part of more than one ‘class’ or group.
Here, as with conservation, the cognitive limitations of centration and lack of reversibility are at the root of the error, in Piaget’s view.
Evaluating Piaget’s theory
The criticisms focus on two issues: claims that he underestimated children’s cognitive capabilities and claims that development is more continuous and less stage-like than he proposed.
Even toddlers show the beginnings of an ability to take others’ perspectives when they discern what they can do to annoy a sibling. By age 4, children switch to shorter, simpler sentences when talking to toddlers or babies, showing a distinctly un-egocentric ability to take the perspective of the younger children.
Theory of mind
The ability to understand thinking processes in one’s self and others and also to understand that others have beliefs, intention and perspectives that are different from one’s own.
Perspective-taking ability advances considerably from age 3 to 6.
This change is vividly demonstrated in research involving false-belief tasks.
Most 3-year-old children answer erroneously that Maxi will look for the chocolate in the new place, where his mother stored it. In contrast, by age 4, most children recognise that Maxi will believe falsely that the chocolate is in the cabinet where he left it. The proportion of children who understand this correctly rises even higher by age 5. By age 6, nearly all children in developed countries solve false-belief tasks easily.
Identify the ways that cultural learning takes place in early childhood.
More than in toddlerhood, young children have the capacity for learning culturally specific skills. A Mayan 5-year-old can readily learn the skills involved in making tortillas, whereas a 2-year-old would not have the necessary learning abilities, motor skills or impulse control.
During early childhood, they acquire the cultural learning necessary for these duties, sometimes through direct instruction but more often through observing and participating in adults’ activities.
A child in an economically developed country might help his parents prepare a grocery shopping list, and in the course of this process learn culturally valued skills such as reading, using lists as tools for organisation and planning, and calculating sums of money.
Children in Western countries are also encouraged to speak up and hold conversations. This is in contrast to cultures from Asia to northern Canada in which silence is valued, especially in children, and children who talk frequently are viewed as immature and low in intelligence.
Children in developed countries are apart from families for more of the day and do not receive as much guided instruction for daily activities as traditional cultures.