Lec 12 and 13 - Flashcards
Smith study 2003 - association
Looking at young adults aged 19-21
Association study - found association between consuming breakfast and better mental health –Measures of ‘health’ included, depression, anxiety, cognitive failures and sleep duration.
Is only correlational and could be the result of confounding factors like home circumstances - these people will be less likely to have breakfast and more likely to have poorer health.
Including potential confounding factors in the analysis (e.g. smoking and alcohol consumption) reduced the statistical significance of the correlations between breakfast and health.
Brown et al 2013-cumulative meta analysis
- missing breakfast is assoicated with obesity - over time more and more evidence has accumulated looking at this association, the estimate of the odds haven’t really changed at all. What has changed, is the confidence intervals have got smaller (more evidence)
the Intervention studies do not clearly confirm or refute proposed effect of eating breakfast on obesity
Levitsky (2005) study - breakfast
Measure energy intake at lunch, snacks and dinner for people who have breakfast vs those who don’t..
If pp miss breakfast, they increase their intake at lunch only and by 135 kcal. But this doesn’t compensate for the missed breakfast (635kcal), so overall there is a decrease in daily energy intake. So this should result in lower body weight.
Your gut is relatively empty regardless of whether or not breakfast is consumed.
So where does the association between missing breakfast and obesity come from?
Obese people are perhaps more likely to miss breakfast. eg start the day by trying to limit intake, but this goal fails and you later consume more. By delaying eating, and eating later on, this gives you less appetite in the morning.
Pollitt et al 1981 - children and cog
Studied the effects of missing breakfast on cognitive performance in children
Children ate at 5pm in the evening and then either consumed breakfast at 8am or missed breakfast. Their cognitive function was assessed at 11am. Tests included matching familiar figures, continuous performance task and hagen central-incidental task
=Missing breakfast increased number of errors on matching familiar test, but only in those who had lower IQ than mean IQ of group
=On incidental recall test, there was a small effect of missing breakfast in which children had improved recall - memory is better for missing breakfast.
what are the issues with breakfast studies?
Blinding
Placebo for ‘no-breakfast’
Publication bias – no effects less likely to get publicated.
Meal size - A large meal acutely impairs performance
Perhaps a small meal is chosen at breakfast so as to avoid impairment (neutral effect)
Not eating breakfast leads to reduced daily energy intake
how much glucose is in the brain and blood?
Brain has a high metabolic rate -
oxidizes 120 g glucose daily, equivalent to about 20% of whole body’s daily energy expenditure
Glucose supplied from blood -
rate of blood supply to brain is high (10 x resting skeletal muscle and same as muscle during vigorous exercise)
Kennedy & Scholey (2000) - glucose
Tested between 9 am and noon, after overnight fast
On one occasion they had a 25g glucose drink, on the other they had a placebo. Tasks were then completed between 27 and 43 minutes after glucose consumption
Tasks included serial threes, serial sevens and word retrieval
=sig effect of glucose on cognitively demanding task only – serial sevens.
what is most amenable to the glucose memory facilitation effect?
verbal episodic memory
possibly suggesting the involvement of the hippocampus in glucose enhancement of memory,
what level is glucose at the start of the day?
4 / 5 mmo/l
blood glucose levels are constant, there are small spikes after eating.
what level of concentration causes glucose uptake to decrease?
Glucose supply to the brain is maintained even during prolonged fasting
Only when blood glucose concentration falls below 2 mmol/l does rate of glucose uptake decrease significantly
what happens to glucose in the body after eating?
Glucose supply to the brain is maintained even during prolonged fasting
-initially from food in the gut (up to 3 h) and glycogen in the liver (3-24 h), then gluconeogenesis (8 h to days )
what tasks is glucose most beneficial for?
high cognitively demanding tasks (Less so in participants with poorer glucose regulatory control (higher blood glucose for longer after a glucose load)
what is the name for high blood glucose? and where is this often seen?
hyperglycaemia
Type 2 diabetes and raised fasting blood glucose levels (hyperglycaemia) and are associated with cognitive impairment
what amino acid synthesises serotonin?
Tryptophan (consumed through protein) it has to be actively transported across blood brain barrier. The amount that gets into brain depends on ratio of blood and other amino acids. It has a fairly low concentraion compared to other amino acids.
what does serotonin effect?
influences satiety, food choice and in relation to mood and alertness/sleepiness, aggression and pain sensitivity.
what drug targets serotonin?
prozac
Carbs vs protein when consumed
and who provides evidence for these effects?
Tryptophan is an amino acid that we consume through protein foods, it has to be actively transported across blood brain barrier. The amount that gets into brain depends on ratio of blood and other amino acids. It has a fairly low concentraion compared to other amino acids. So a diet high in protein, reduces the amount of Tryptophan that enters the brain (due to competing amino acids).
When carbohydrates are consumed, the opposite happens. Large amino acids are taken out of blood into muscle, apart from Tryptophan which remains in blood because it doesn’t dissolve and doesn’t get taken into muscles. Therefore there is an increase in Tryptophan to the brain after carbs.
Liberman et al 1986 study provides evidence of this
Lieberman et al. (1986) study- Pp fed a high protein meal or high sugar meal or high starch meal – they measure ratio of …
Pp fed a high protein meal or high sugar meal or high starch meal – they measure ratio of Tryptophan to amino acids after consumption (measure every hour)
These were extreme meal manipulations (eg 160g of sugar)
=they demonstrate expected effects of trypotophan: LNAA, protein causes decline in ratio relative to starch and sugar which increase it. This isn’t an immediate increase it happens between 1 and 2 hours
Spring et al 1982- Higher Carb meal in males vs females
Higher Carb meal which increases serotonergic activity, increases sleepiness in females but found opposite in males.
Higher protein had higher sleepiness in males.
Performance is impaired in older people in the afternoon in people who had a higher carb meal
Pivonka & Grunewald. (1990) effect of sugar
Consumption of water versus low calorie drink versus 50g sucrose drink
=there is a small but reliable effect of sugar increasing sleepiness after consumption within 30 minutes.
This is not in line with Liberman’s carb craving findings, perhaps the mechanism for this behaviour is therefore something else.
Wurtman & Wurtman. (1989) = carbohydrate-craving obesity’
Proposed circle relationship between mood, food intake and brain serotonin in ‘carbohydrate-craving obesity’
having low mood as a result of vulnerability to depression (under-functioning serotonergic system) can be relieved if one consumes carbohydrates. Or perhaps low mood causes you to crave carb – they don’t say whether this is conscious or not.
Liberman 1986 - carb cravers
Measured mood before and 2 h after consumption of a high-carbohydrate lunch in obese ‘carbohydrate cravers’ and obese non-carbohydrate cravers
=noncarbohydrate cravers reported feeling considerably less alert and more fatigued and sleepy, while carbohydrate cravers described little or no change.
=noncarbohydrate cravers experienced an increase in depression, while carbohydrate cravers reported feeling less depressed.
markus et al 2000 - high stress proneness
Subjects with high stress proneness or low stress proneness participated in a controllable- and uncontrollable-stress experiment during either a protein-rich- carbohydrate-poor diet (40% CHO, 26% protein) or carb-rich and protein poor (66% CHO, 4% protein).
= feelings of depression became higher under the protein-rich carb-poor diet condition in high stress prone
=dep was lower in carb-rich PP group