Lecture 2 - Individual Differences in Colour Vision (Colour Vision 1) Flashcards
(42 cards)
Why does colour vision exist? Name one of the uses of colour and where it appears. (3)
Colour is used in communication.
Colour can indicate ripeness/quality of food, someone’s health (i.e., pale, feverish, etc.), symbolism in safety, and in aesthetics (clothes, appearance, decoration, etc.)
Which two colours are often used symbolically as signals?
Red and Green.
Name another use of colour towards survival.
Camouflage uses colours of the environment to help blend in and hide from predators/prey.
What else can colour provide us in terms of environmental information? Give an example of what happens when we do not have access to colour.
Colour allows us to segment and differentiate between objects in space.
For example, viewing a grey-scale image limits the information we can process in a scene. We may not identify which flowers are in a field if we had no access to their colours.
What is the difference between humans and dogs - and most other species - in their colour vision?
Humans and primates are trichromatic. This means we have three cones which process colour vision. Dogs and most other species are dichromatic, meaning they only have two types of cones which process colour.
Fish have tetrachromacy, and most insects have pentachromacy. Explain what this means and provide an example.
Fish have tetra-chromatic vision, which means they have four cones. Pentachromacy means five cones. The peacock mantis shrimp has 12 types of cones - allowing them to perceive more colours than we could ever hope to imagine.
TRUE or FALSE. We all see the same colours. Our eyes, brains, and the functions/mechanisms which allow us to see colour do not matter - all colours are the same.
FALSE. Colour is a construct. Colour vision depends heavily on the eyes and brain of the beholder. It also depends on the machinery and functions within the eye which code for colours.
We are constantly constructing a scene based on what we can and cannot process. Thus, individual differences can often arise in our perception of colour.
What is central to colour vision, and what happens to colours when this changes?
Illumination is central to colour vision. Lower illumination limits the colours we can perceive.
How do we perceive colour?
We perceive colour from the reflective spectrum. This is the number of wavelengths, or frequencies of light, reflected into our eyes.
Which colour is most reflected in most individuals? How do we see this colour?
Red is reflected the most.
Reflected wavelengths allow us to perceive a colour as ‘red’, but the colour red doesn’t exist as a physical property. ‘Red’ is just different frequencies of light being reflected into our eyes.
What are the three types of cones? How do they differ?
The three cones are Blue (S), Green (M), Red (L). These cones are more sensitive to different spectral frequencies. Sensitivity depends on size.
Which two frequencies and cones overlap, and which two have a larger gap between them?
Red and green frequencies - and therefore red (L) and green (M) cones - overlap much more than green and blue frequencies or M and S cones.
Where are blue (S) cones manufactured or produced?
The gene which produces S, or blue cones, sits on chromosome 7.
Where are the genes which code for green (M) and red (L) cones located? What is their special quality?
M and L cones are made on the X chromosome at position q128. They are X-linked on this sex chromosome.
What is a defining feature of the fovea, an indentation on the retina where visual acuity is the highest?
The fovea does not contain any S or blue cones. As such, it is considered ‘blue-blind’.
Which spectral frequencies is the eye most sensitive to, and why is this the case?
Most of the spectral sensitivity in the eye relates to red and green cones. We often have twice as many red cones as green cones.
TRUE or FALSE. We have no variability in our spectral sensitivity to red or green. Meaning, we all see red or green in the same way.
FALSE. Our spectral sensitivity to red and green frequencies may vary significantly.
What are the two main causes of variability in red and green spectral sensitivity?
1) The spectral sensitivity of the green (M) and red (L) cones themselves.
2) The number of M and L cones an individual has.
Explain the principle of univariance in relation to colour vision. (5)
The principle of univariance suggests that any single cone in the visual system is colourblind by itself. In other words, any combination of wavelengths and intensity can produce the same response in every cone.
Each cone, therefore, needs to compare inputs between spectral sensitivities. Ganglion cells exist to compare these inputs. This is to distinguish between spectral frequencies and wavelengths, resulting in perceiving different colours.
Explain the blue-yellow channel of colour vision. This is otherwise known as S vs. (L + M).
The S cone becomes excitatory, which fires a signal. This signal is compared to inhibitory L and M cones by bi-stratified ganglion cells.
YES or NO. Is the S vs. (L + M) or blue-yellow pathway recent? Does it code for spatial information?
NO. The S vs. (L + M) or blue-yellow pathway is ancient. It does not code for spatial information, it only codes for colour.
What is the blue-yellow channel sensitive to? What can happen if the blue-yellow channel is damaged?
The blue-yellow channel is sensitive to toxic substances. This includes drugs, alcohol, and nicotine. An individual may develop a loss in the blue-yellow channel if these substances are abused.
Explain the red-green channel of colour vision. This is known as L vs. (L + M). Is this channel connected to spatial information?
The L cones become excitatory. It then fires an action potential, which is compared by midget ganglion cells to the surrounding inhibitory L (Red) and M (Green) cones. This channel is connected to spatial information.
Different to the blue-yellow colour system, which uses bi-stratified ganglion cells, what other functions do the midget ganglion cells of the red-green colour system have? (2)
Midget cells control spatial vision and processing the peripherals or edges of an object.