Lecture 4 - Group Processes Flashcards
What is a group?
- “A group is two or more individuals in face-to-face interaction, each aware of his or her membership in the group, each aware of the others who belong to the group, and each aware of their positive interdependence as they strive to achieve mutual goals”.
(Johnson & Johnson, 1987, p.8)
Why do we study groups?
- “If we take care of the individual, psychologically speaking, the groups will take care of themselves” (Allport, 1924, p.9)
- If we study individuals, we will know about groups, because a group is just a collection of individuals?
What are the types of groups?
Lickel et al. (2000)
Groups can be lots of different things
Strong interpersonal relationships
- Families
- Small groups of close friends
Formed to fulfil task(s)
- Committees
- Work groups
- Together for a particular purpose
Groups based on large social categories
E.g. women, Americans
Groups based on weak social relationships
- E.g. people who enjoy Taylor Swift’s music, people from the same local area
Transitory groups
- E.g. people waiting at the bus stop; people in the queue at the bank
Minimal groups
- Tajel, Billig, Bundy and Flament (1971, replicated by Allen & Wilder, 1975)
- ‘Minimal groups’; split randomly into two-groups
- People allocated more money to their ‘own’ group than the other group, and the effect could not be explained by:
- Self-interest (as they didn’t get a share)
- Existing friendships (as allocation was random)
- Demonstrates how easily bias (and groups), i.e. in-group favouritism, can develop
What did early work show about social facilitation?
- Triplett (1898) was the first to ask these sorts of questions
- Observed track cyclists and found performances were faster when:
- Timed alone
- Timed and racing alongside other cyclists
- Hypothesised that the presence of the audience, particularly in a competition, ‘energised’ performance on motor tasks (made them do better)
- Triplett tested his hypothesis using a ‘fishing line’ apparatus and found that children performed better when racing against each other than when alone
What has other work shown about social facilitation?
- Allport (1920) termed this phenomenon ‘Social Facilitation’
- He suggested a more generalised effect: ‘Mere Presence’
- Mere presence is defined as an “entirely passive and unresponsive audience that is only physically present” (Hogg & Vaughan, 2014, p.275)
- Improvement in performance due to the mere presence of others as co-actors or passive audience
- Not just humans: Kangaroos, monkeys and horses eat more and run faster when other members of their species are doing the same thing (e.g., Dindo, et al., 2009; Pays, et al., 2009)
Social facilitation vs inhibition
- However, shortly after the discovery, some studies showed how the presence of others can impaired performance for both humans and animals (see review by Bond & Titus, 1983), known as social inhibition
- For example:
- Complex task (e.g. typing name backwards), done more slowly in the presence of other people than alone (Schmitt, et al., 1986)
- Men take longer to urinate when someone is standing immediately beside them at a urinal than alone (Middlemist et al., 1976)
What does Zajonc’s (1965) drive theory suggest?
- Argued mere presence of others creates an increase in arousal and energises ‘dominant response’:
- ‘Dominant response’ is that what is typically done in that situation i.e., a well-learnt/habitual response
- When people are anxious, they tend to do better on easy tasks (already good at) and worse on difficult ones (that they normally struggle at)
- If the dominant response is correct (easy), then performance will be facilitated
- If the dominant response is incorrect (difficult), then performance will be inhibited
Social facilitation/inhibition
- “An improvement in the performance of well-learnt/easy tasks and a deterioration in the performance of poorly-learnt/difficult tasks in the mere presence of members of the same species” (Hogg & Vaughan, 2014, p. 275)
- It does seem plausible in some ways – joggers appear to run faster with others
- However, some situations with others come in competitive contexts e.g., a race with other runners, where there is an audience watching/judging their every move. What happens?
What does Cottrell’s (1972) evaluation apprehension theory suggest?
- We learn about social reward/punishment contingencies (e.g., approval and disapproval) based on others’ evaluation
- Perception of an ‘evaluating’ audience creates arousal, not mere presence
- Social facilitation is an acquired effect based on perceived evaluations of others
How does the study by Cottrell et al. (1968) support the evaluation apprehension hypothesis?
- Cottrell et al. (1968) supported this hypothesis in an experiment with 3 audience conditions:
- (1) Blindfolded (cannot see participant)
- (2) Merely present (passive and uninterested)
- (3) Attentive audience
- Tasks were well learned (i.e. easy)
- Social facilitation was found when the audience was perceived to be evaluative (attentive); wanting to perform well for their audience worked in their favour (2 vs 3 should not differ if ‘mere presence’)
How does the research by Markus (1978) contradict Cottrell?
Time taken to dress in familiar clothes (easy task, own clothes)/unfamiliar clothes (difficult task, lab coat and unfamiliar shoes) as a function of social presence
- 3 conditions:
- (1) alone
- (2) in the presence of an inattentive audience
- (3) in the presence of an attentive audience
- Attentive audience speeded up performance in easy task
- Inattentive and attentive not much different in difficult task (performed more slowly)
What did Schmitt et al. (1986) find about evaluation apprehension?
- Schmitt et al. (1986) asked participants to type either their name or a code backwards on a computer
- Mere presence of others made people perform the simple task quicker and the difficult task slower
- However, adding in an evaluation apprehension condition made little difference to the typing speed
- Found no difference in typing speed
- It seems that evaluation apprehension is sometimes helpful but sometimes unnecessary for social facilitation
What does the distraction-conflict theory suggest?
- People become distracted, focusing (“drive”) on what others are doing (i.e., evaluating them), and perform worse
- E.g. Sanders et al. (1978) had participants’ complete an easy or difficult digit task, but:
- (1) Alone
- (2) Someone doing the same task or someone doing a different task
- People performed worse when someone did the same thing as them (i.e. more distraction)
- Not just others – Sanders (1981) showed that bursts of light could similarly affect social facilitation
Does it matter what is evaluating you?
- Siemon (2023) examined whether using AI-based idea evaluation led to evaluation apprehension
- Finnish participant presented an idea to either Alan or Phillip
- Participants express less evaluation apprehension when presenting their idea to Alan (AI) than Phillip
- The results show that when humans are involved in evaluating an idea, people tend to feel concerned
What is social loafing?
Social loafing (loss of motivation termed ‘social loafing’ (Latane et al., 1979) “Reduction in individual effort when working on a collective task compared with working either alone”)
What did Ringelmann (1913, 1927) find about social loafing?
- Ringelmann (1913, 1927) found that men pulling on a rope attached to a dynamometer exerted less force than the number of people in the group
- Reasons for the effect:
- Coordination loss: as group size inhibits movement, distraction, and jostling
- Motivation loss: participants did not try as hard; less motivated
What did Ingham et al. (1974) find about social loafing?
- Ingham et al. (1974) = investigated this with ‘real groups’ and ‘pseudo groups’ pulling on a rope; participant blind-folded
- Real group = groups of varying size
- Pseudo-group = only one true participant, rest were confederates who did not pull at all
- Pseudo-groups put in more effort
What did Latané et al. (1979) find about social loafing?
- Latané et al. (1979) supported this through clapping, shouting, and cheering tasks
- Recorded amount of cheering/clapping noise made per person [blind folded] reduced by:
- 29% in 2-person groups
- 49% in 4-person groups
- 60% in 6-person groups