Midterm 3 Flashcards
(108 cards)
Growth Mindset Paper
- A problem with complimenting kids solely on their effort and not on their ability as well can backfire, and can communicate that they’re not very capable and therefore unlikely to succeed at future tasks.
- The praise itself, whether about effort or ability, is the main problem however
- It’s a verbal reward; research has shown that kids end up less interested in whatever they were rewarded or praised for doing because now their goal is to just get praise as opposed to actually doing well on something due to interest in the subject
- Praise also communicates that our approval is conditional on the child’s continuing to impress us or do what we say
- What kids really need is unconditional support
- Another problem with praise is that if kids are preoccupied with how well they’re doing in school, then their interest in what they’re doing may suffer; a study found that adopting a growth mindset wasn’t helpful for students whose self-worth was dependent upon their performance, and they were more likely to ‘self-handicap’
- The biggest problem with this idea of growth-mindset encouraging praise is focusing on mindset itself, believing that it emphasizes that there’s something that we need to change about ourselves, and that the environment/condition can’t be changed
- Related to the fundamental attribution error: paying so much attention to personality and attitudes that we overlook how profoundly the social environment affects what we d and who we are; it causes us to ignore the politics behind things and not see how we can improve our conditions
Overall message: praise, whether it’s encouraging a growth mindset or not, causes kids to become focused on achievement as opposed to being focused on the actually learning itself. It also encourages the idea that our conditions/environment are fixed, and that we can only focus on fixing/improving ourselves as opposed to improving both ourselves AND the conditions we’re in, since the conditions we’re in can be a bit toxic (think ‘culture of poverty’); some educators would rather convince students that they need to adopt a more positive attitude than address the quality of the curriculum or the pedagogy
Fundamental Attribution Error
- From growth mindset paper
- Def: paying so much attention to personality and attitudes that we overlook how profoundly the social environment affects what we do and who we are
Asher and Paquette: Loneliness and Peer Relations in Childhood Paper
- Loneliness inchildren is influenced by howwell accepted they are by peers,whether they are overtly victim-ized, whether they have friends,and the durability and qualityof their best friendships
- Loneliness definition: the cognitive awareness of a deficiency in one’s social and personal relationships, and the ensuing affective reactions of sadness, emptiness, or longing
- It is possible to have many friends and still feel lonely; it’s also possible to be poorly accepted by the peer group or lack friends and not feel lonely
- Chronic loneliness is associated with various indices of maladjustment in adolescents and adults, including dropping out of school, depression, alcoholism, and medical problems
- Kids start to have an at least rudimentary idea of loneliness at age 5 or 6; understand that loneliness consists of solitude and a depressed affect
- There is a consistent association between peer acceptance and loneliness; kids who are poorly accepted report experiencing higher levels of loneliness in kindergartners all the way up to middle school students
- There are distinct subgroups within rejected children however; withdrawn-rejected children consistently report greater loneliness than aggressive-rejected children. One factor that may account for variability in rejected children’s feelings of loneliness is overt victimization, as highly dislike children who are victimized are more likely to report elevated loneliness
- There is no evidence to date that the number of friends children have relates to loneliness, but it is important for children to have friendships that endure
- Children who make new friends but do not maintain their friendships experience higher levels of loneliness
- Children who participate in high-quality friendships experience less loneliness than other children
- Children who have ‘idealized’ views of their friends, like that their friends will never let them down and will always be there for them are likely to experience disappointments when outsiders would think the friendship is going well
- Children who believe that conflict is a sign of impending dissolution of a friendship are likely to experience higher levels of loneliness
Kuhl on the Linguistic genius of babies
- Did the head-turn task, in which babies were conditioned to turn their heads when they heard a different sound
- Did this with two English language sounds that aren’t important to distinguish between in Japanese, and tested American and Japanese children
- At 6-8 months, both groups of kids were equally as likely to respond correctly
- At 10-12 months, the American children had become better at responding, and the Japanese children had become worse
- During this 2 month time frame, babies are listening to us and taking statistics on the sounds they’re hearing in our language, seeing different distributions of sounds, and using those statistics to become more or less sensitive to certain sounds
- Repeated experiment with American infants and Taiwanese infants and saw the same results, and then did a test in which in that critical time period, American babies were exposed to Mandarin for 12 sessions. Babies who were exposed to Mandarin were then as good as babies in Taiwan when given these sessions
- Researchers then wanted to know what the presence of a human being there and talking to them had, so they repeated the experiment, but this time with the kids getting their exposure through a video, and babies who had just audio exposure. The audio and video group showed no learning; babies need humans to be there and be present for them to take their statistics.
- Used MEG machines to record babies while listening to different languages and see their brain activity
Symbols
- Systems for representing thoughts, feelings, and knowledge, and communicating them to others
- Language is the most powerful symbolic system
Generativity of Language
- We can take the finite set of words in our vocabulary and put them together in an infinite number of sentences and express and infinite number of ideas
Four different components of language
- Phonology: perception and production of speech sounds
- Semantics: how we segment words and learn their meaning
- Pragmatics: the context in which language is used
- Syntax: arrangement of words and phrases to create well-formed sentences
Phonemes
- the elements of sound in speech
- Ex: when –> wh/e/n = 3 phonemes
- English uses just 45 of the 200 sounds found in all languages
- Perception of phonemes is not continuous, but is categorical
What a child needs to be able to do to master phonology
- Discriminate the sounds of their native language
2. Categorize the sounds of their native language
McGurk effect
- Example of phonological perception being categorical as opposed to continuous
- Shows that speech perception results from an interaction between sound and vision
- If we see a video of someone saying ‘far’ next to a video of someone saying ‘bar’ but hear the same word (either bar or far) over both videos, we will think that the individual is saying the word that they’re mouthing, and the word we hear will change as we look back and forth between the videos; we’ll only hear ‘bar’ or ‘far’ though, we won’t hear some intermediary sound
How Phoneme Perception Develops
- Babies learn to NOT tell apart sounds that aren’t important in their language
- Shows that phoneme perception is also an example of perceptual narrowing, as well as the ‘use it or lose it’ function; the development of phoneme perception in infants is similar to their development of facial recognition
Conditioned Head-Turn Study
- Used to measure perceptual narrowing of phonemes in babies
- Conditioned babies to look to the side when they heard a different sound
- Ex: ba..ba..ba..da
- Infants at 6 months were able to tell the difference between the sounds (‘universal listeners’)
- At 10-12 months, infants lost the ability to distinguish between sounds that weren’t important to distinguish between in their native language
- This loss of sensitivity (roughly) coincides with the onset of word learning (6 months to 1 year)
Early vocalizations
- At 6-8 weeks, babies coo, which is the production of drawn-out vowel sounds
- They also have an increased awareness that vocalization elicits response from others
- By practicing vowels, they’re gaining motor control of their facial muscles
- At 6-10 months, babies start to babble, which is repeated strings of syllables (ba ba ba)
- This babbling gradually converges on sounds, intonation, and rhythm of their native language
- Congenitally deaf babies will do manual babbling if exposed to sign language
- Other animals babble as well, like birds and bats
What children need to grasp to learn word meanings
- What are words (word segmentation)
2. What do the words mean
Semantics Step 1: Word Segmentation
- Where do words start/end
- Attending to pauses generally won’t help grasp word segmentation, since human speech occurs without breaks
- Statistical learning helps babies segment words, as they pay attention to transitional? probabilities to guess what are individual words or not
Mondegreens
- Mishearing or misinterpretation of a phrase; occurs a lot with song lyrics
Word Segmentation Experiment
- Took 8 month old babies and put them through a familiarity phase and a test phase
- In the familiarity phase, they played four made-up words for the babies being repeated over and over again in the span of 2 minutes at the rate at which normal speech is spoken (no pauses between words)
- Their only cues at word boundaries were thus to use transitional probability, which is how likely each syllable is to follow the previous one. It’s higher with actual words, of course
- The test phase then involved the babies listening to either a full word or part of a full word from the fake words they had just heard
- The babies were able to discriminate between ‘words’ and ‘parts-of-words’, showing that infants are sensitive to which syllables repeatedly occurred together
Transitional Probability
- In semantics, it’s the likelihood that a particular syllable is to follow the previous one
- Is higher in actual words than syllables between words (ex: higher in ac-tual than tual-words)
- What babies use to determine what are words and what aren’t when hearing speech
Semantics Step 2: What do words mean
- Kids have comprehension of highly familiar words at 6 months old (they’ll look towards an object you label, like ‘dog’)
- Kids say their first words around 10-15 months old
- A vocabulary explosion occurs around 18 months, at which children learn words fast, and can learn through just a single exposure via fast-mapping
- Word learning is not straightforward, and kids have to make some default assumptions when learning words
- They assume that the word refers to the object as a whole and not a portion, place, or property.
Ex: if you point to a rabbit and say ‘rabbit’, they assume the whole rabbit is a rabbit, not that its’ tail is a rabbit - They assume the new word refers only to objects of that shape, and that each thing will only have one shape (a rabbit is only a rabbit, not a rabbit and bunny)
- Kids also infer what a new word means by ruling out objects they already know (ex. show them a rubber duck and an unknown object and ask which is the ‘koba’, they’re going to select the unknown object)
- Linguistic context is also useful to help kids understand word meanings
Fast Mapping
- The technique/name for when kids are able to learn a word after just one exposure to the word, even if the word is only referred to indirectly
Fast Mapping Koba Study
- Kids were shown an object that was unfamiliar to them and were told it was a ‘koba’
- Kids were then brought back a week later and a month later and shown various objects, asked to point out the ‘koba’, and they remembered
Linguistic context
- Useful in semantics
- the syntactic form of a word (noun, verb, etc) can help children figure out what a word is referring to
Ex: If a person is shown mixing up some small objects with their hands in a bowl/container and are told ‘this is a sib’, the kids will think the sib is the container - If they are told ‘he is sibbing’, they’ll think the action he’s doing is sibbing
- If they’re told “this is some sib’, they’ll think the objects are sibs.
- Shows that children are sensitive to grammatical cues
Word Production in Babies
- Usually say their first word at 10-12 months
- When they first start speaking, they use one word utterances/phrases to communicate (ex. ‘water’ for ‘I want more water’)
- Kids will first go through underextension, in which they think a name of a group refers to only that specific type of object in the group as opposed to the group itself( ex. ‘flower’ refers to only roses, and not sunflowers or any other type of flower)
- Kids will then go through overextension since what they want to talk about quickly out-strips their vocabulary (ex. will use the word ‘ball’ to refer to a ball, a balloon, a marble, and apple, etc)
- The number of words a child knows is intimately related to the number of words they hear
Pragmatics and Word Learning
- Pragmatics involves paying attention to social cues/context and to where the speaker is attending
- Ex: a toddler can use the speaker’s gaze to determine what the novel word they used is referring to
- Understanding the speaker’s emotion can help as well