Problem 2 Flashcards
Temperament
- refers to an infant’s inherent emotional disposition (e.g., emotionality, activity level, sociability).
- believed to have biological roots and shows stability in infancy.
- personality development is influenced by both genetic factors (temperament) and environmental factors.
Temperament-Based Components of Personality and Early Development
- early differences in temperament lead to variations in personality.
- key components: emotionality (intensity of emotional reactions), activity level, sociability.
- temperament affects later behavior, social interactions, and adaptability.
Common temperamental categories
easy, difficult, and slow-to-warm-up children.
Goodness of fit
- the match between a child’s temperament and their environment (e.g., parenting style).
- good fit leads to better emotional regulation and development, while a poor fit can lead to emotional or behavioral challenges.
- parenting styles that align with the child’s temperament enhance emotional well-being.
Emotions
- transient states with physiological, cognitive, and behavioral components
- physiological responses (e.g., heart rate) signal emotional states.
- cognitive processes influence how we appraise situations, affecting emotional responses
- behavioral processes involve actions (e.g., crying, smiling) that communicate emotions.
Development of Emotions in Infancy
- newborns show rudimentary emotions like distress and contentment.
- positive and negative emotions emerge early; more complex emotions develop over time.
- by 3 months, emotions like joy, surprise, and distress appear.
- 6 months: emotions like fear and anger differentiate.
Primary emotions
joy, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, disgust (universal and cross-cultural).
Basic Emotions and Evolutionary Function
- basic emotions serve adaptive roles (e.g., fear warns of danger, joy encourages approach).
- these emotions are innate and shared across cultures.
- infants’ emotional expressions communicate their internal states to others, prompting caregivers to respond.
Approaches to Studying Emotional Development
- facial expressions
- Darwin’s research: studied how facial expressions signal emotional states across species.
- emotions are functional: they mobilize actions to achieve goals (e.g., anger drives action, sadness leads to seeking comfort).
Differentiation of Emotions in Infancy
- early emotions are undifferentiated (global positive or negative states).
- over time, emotions become more refined (e.g., sadness and anger separate).
Infants’ Emotional Expressions
- positive emotions: smiles and joy (e.g., when mastering a skill or engaging socially).
- negative emotions: initially difficult to distinguish (e.g., all negative states may appear as distress).
- as infants grow, they differentiate fear from other negative emotions by around 6 months.
Easy Children (40-50% of infants)
- General Characteristics: These children are typically calm, adaptable, and easygoing.
- emotional Reactivity: They tend to have regular biological rhythms (e.g., sleep, feeding schedules) and are predictable in their behavior.
- adaptability: Easy children adjust easily to changes in routine or environment.
- approach to New Experiences: They are generally curious and show a positive attitude toward new situations.
- emotional Regulation: They are usually able to regulate their emotions and rarely get overly distressed.
- parenting Fit: Easy children tend to do well with most parenting styles and often help create a positive parent-child relationship due to their ease of management.
Difficult Children (10-15% of infants)
- general Characteristics: These children are more likely to show irregularity in their biological rhythms (e.g., difficulty sleeping, feeding, etc.).
- emotional Reactivity: They often exhibit intense emotional responses to stimuli, both positive and negative.
- adaptability: Difficult children are typically less adaptable to new situations, and changes in routine can be more challenging for them.
- approach to New Experiences: They tend to show hesitation or avoidance toward unfamiliar situations or people.
- emotional Regulation: These children may have difficulty calming down once upset and may need more effort to soothe.
- parenting Fit: Difficult children may require more patience and consistent caregiving. They might benefit from a structured and predictable environment to reduce frustration.
Slow-to-Warm-Up Children (15-20% of infants)
- general characteristics: These children are characterized by a gradual approach to new situations or people, appearing shy or reserved at first.
- emotional Reactivity: They may show mild distress when faced with new or unfamiliar situations but do not react as intensely as difficult children.
- adaptability: Slow-to-warm-up children tend to need more time to adjust to changes in their environment, but once they become familiar with a situation, they often respond in a more positive way.
- approach to New Experiences: They are cautious and take their time to warm up to new people or experiences, but they eventually become more comfortable.
- emotional Regulation: They usually have moderate levels of emotional intensity but may take longer to calm down when distressed compared to easy children.
- parenting Fit: Slow-to-warm-up children benefit from gentle encouragement and gradual exposure to new situations. A supportive and reassuring parenting style helps them adjust without overwhelming them.
What is a possible reason why fear emerges later than sadness and anger?
Fear may require more complex mental representations to assess a potential threat, which might be beyond the cognitive capabilities of very young infants.
How are basic emotions different from complex emotions?
Basic emotions (e.g., anger, fear, sadness) emerge early in life and are often linked to survival mechanisms, while complex emotions (e.g., guilt, shame, pride) emerge later and often involve more intricate cognitions and social awareness.
What are self-conscious emotions, and when do they typically appear?
self-conscious emotions, such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and pride, require a sense of self in relation to others and usually begin to emerge around ages 1½ to 2.
What brain structure is linked with basic emotions?
Basic emotions are closely linked to the amygdala, a brain structure involved in emotional memory formation.
What are the differences between shame and guilt as complex emotions?
- Shame involves wanting to hide due to loss of respect from others.
- Guilt involves feeling the need to remedy a situation after a specific failure or wrongdoing.
What is embarrassment, and how does it develop in young children?
Embarrassment typically occurs when individuals feel they are receiving unwanted attention or have violated social norms.
It can develop as early as 2 years old and is linked to the emerging ability to understand the self in social contexts.
How do Machiavellian emotions in infants function?
These emotions (e.g., using facial expressions to elicit caregiver responses) may be displayed early in life to manipulate social interactions, even if the infant does not fully experience the associated emotion.
Q: What study suggests that infants have a basic sense of morality
- Research using geometric shapes (helper vs. hinderer) found that infants as young as 6 months preferred to interact with helpful figures, suggesting early emotional responses to moral behavior.
What is the developmental sequence for emotions in infancy?
Contentment, interest, and distress at birth.
Sadness and disgust at around 3 months.
Anger at around 4 months.
Fear around 6 months.
Self-conscious emotions (like pride and shame) develop between 1½ and 2 years.