Top Hat module 5 Flashcards

1
Q

Attention according to William James

A

“Everyone knows what attention is. It is taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seems several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration of consciousness are of its essence. It implies a withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others.” (quote from 1890)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Inattentional blindness

A

The failure to perceive an object or event that occurs in plain sight. The failure is not due to visual impairments, but to lack of attention (we are unable to perceive information outside of the attentional spotlight). Where you focus your attention determines what properties of a scene you are and aren’t able to report on.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Change-blindess

A

A form of inattentional blindness in which people have difficulty detecting the difference between two versions of a picture that are alternately presented. In the case of change blindness, researchers can measure the effects of different variables on the probability that someone will notice the change at a given point in the sequence.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Measuring change-blindness

A

One interesting question that researchers have looked at using this paradigm is the relationship between where people are directly fixating and the likelihood of noticing a change. Most of the time, people don’t notice the change until they look directly at the location where the change is taking place. However, several studies have suggested that people can miss changes even when they are looking directly at a changing object. This can explain why people fall for magic tricks, even though they focus their entire attention on figuring it out.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Why are we prone to change-blindness?

A

It takes a great deal of processing to go from a flat, two-dimensional image to recognizable objects situated in a three-dimensional world, and we simply don’t have sufficient processing resources to compute everything all at once, especially when you include all the other senses we are constantly using (touch, audition…)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Inattentional deafness

A

A phenomenon in which auditory information is not perceived when a different high-load task is being performed.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Selective attention

A

A form of attentional control in which a single data stream (e.g., an object or voice) is processed while others are ignored. Because of the selective nature of our attention, many researchers liken it to a filter, in which the desired information makes it through, while other information is discarded.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Cocktail party effect

A

The ability to attend to a specific voice in an environment in which other competing voices are present as well.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Dichotic listening task (Cherry, 1953)

A

An experimental task designed to assess selective attention. Participants are presented, via headphones, with two different audio streams to each of the two ears and tasked with repeating only one of the streams while ignoring the other. Cherry found that his participants could identify whether the unattended channel’s message was spoken by a male or female but could remember little else. In general, Cherry concluded that unless something changed that made the new message physically distinct, the attentional filter would block its contents.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Further research by Moray (1959) on the dichotic listening task

A

Moray found that a word could be repeated upwards of 30 times and still not be recognized by participants on a later test. This research on selective attention helped inform theories of when our attentional filter starts to block information and prevent it from further processing.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Early-selection models

A

A model of attention that posits that unattended information is filtered based on basic physical characteristics without processing meaning. For example, at a cocktail party, because understanding the meaning of words requires additional processing, the unattended conversations would only be so much meaningless noise to your brain.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Broadbent’s filter model (Broadbent, 1958)

A

A high amount of sensory information enters the nervous system. This high informational load is held briefly in sensory memory . Then, selective attention determines which (smaller) portion of the information makes its way through to the “detector,” which is the mechanism that processes the meaning of the information (this is what makes it an early-selection model: meaning is processed only after the filter has been applied).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Evidence against early-selection models

A

In Moray’s (1959) study mentioned above, participants were often able to process unattended information if their name was spoken in the ignored channel. In addition, research by Grey and Weddeburn (1960) found that if a meaningful narrative was played in such a manner that each successive word alternated between the ears, people would follow the narrative. For example, one stimulus presented a letter, alternating between ears. At the same time, a sequence of numbers was presented to the opposite ear. Participants usually reported the content of the letter rather than the numbers. Both of these results demonstrate that, under some circumstances, the meaning of an unattended stimulus is processed.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Late-selection models

A

A model of attention that posits that unattended information is first processed in terms of its meaning, and then filtered based on irrelevance to the current task. What attentional filtering is based on, according to these models, is not simple physical characteristics, but whether it fits semantically with attended information.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Attenuator model of attention

A

A theory of attention (proposed by Anne Treisman, 1964), in which unattended stimuli are processed but at a reduced level relative to attended stimuli. It’s a compromise between the early- and late-selection models. This theory states that there is some filtering of the incoming stimulus based on its physical properties. However, some of the information makes its way through the filter.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Attenuator model of attention - what makes info important enough to be processed?

A

The idea is that some portion of the signal makes its way through for further processing, although it is reduced (or “attenuated”). The meaning of this reduced signal may be identified if it sufficiently matches some high-priority word (such as your name) or an expected item (e.g., a word that may be more predicted by the previous words). Even reduced signals may be identified if they meet these criteria. We may think of a filtered stimulus as having reduced resolution compared with those that are attended.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Mackay’s twist on the dichotic listening task (1973)

A

Mackay (1973) altered the dichotic listening task by having participants focus on sentences with a potentially ambiguous meaning, while in the unattended channel a word was repeatedly played that would provide context to the attended sentence. Mackay was interested in how participants’ memory of the attended sentence might change based on the word repeated in the unattended channel. He found that the “unconscious” information was somehow affecting participants’ conscious perception. These results indicate that our attentional filter is much more flexible than once believed

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Attentional load

A

A measure of how much processing resources are needed in order to perform a task. The idea of attentional load suggests that sometimes unattended stimuli may be processed even if we are trying to filter them out (ex. hearing a bag of chips at the theater: even if you are trying to just watch the movie, there may be some processing resources left over that end up processing the unwanted sound of the snack bag. ).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Eriksen flanker task (1974)

A

A technique used to study attention in which an irrelevant distractor is included alongside experimental stimuli in order to see whether the distractor is processed, increasing reaction time. This research demonstrated that when trying to search for a target letter among distractors, the difficulty varies by what is flanking, or distracting, for the target item. Participants can’t help but process the flankers. When they were consistent with the same response as the target, it caused no decrease in performance. But when the distractors indicated a different response, they created conflict, leading to longer reaction times.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Lavie’s version of the flanker task (1995)

A

In this case, participants have to search for a letter “O” in the two rings of letters. Next to the rings is a flanker letter that is not relevant to the task, but that may serve to distract the participant if it is incompatible. There is a “low-load” (the letters in the ring are uniform, so it’s easy to spot the target) task, and a “high-load task” (every letter is different).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Results form Lavie’s study

A

Lavie found that the incompatible flanker only caused an increased reaction time in the low-load condition but not in the high-load conditions. She concluded that in the low-load condition, the fact that the task is not difficult means that there are processing resources left over and that these resources end up processing the flanker, even when it is not wanted, leading to distraction. In the high-load condition, however, the central task of finding the target is difficult and there are few processing resources left over. Therefore, the flanker is not processed and does not cause a distraction.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Green and Bavelier’s twist on Lavie’s research (2003)

A

They recruited people who had a lot of experience playing first-person video games versus people with little to no experience, and compared their performance in Lavie’s flanker tasks. The non-gamers showed the same pattern of results as found by Lavie: distraction from the incompatible flanker in the low-load condition but not the high-load. However, the gamer showed distraction from the flanker in both conditions. This means that people with video game experience are better able to distribute their attention outside of the central task, even under high-load conditions, leading them to process the distracting flanker.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

So, what model is correct?

A

The role of attentional load suggests that neither the early or late theories are completely right or wrong. Attention may have a movable filter that can be applied more strictly based on the demands of a task.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Automatic processing

A

Processing that happens even without the allocation of selective attention, typically for highly familiar stimuli or tasks (often the result of practice and learning).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Stroop effect

A

Stroop (1935) found that people took longer to do a color-naming task when the words spelled out by the colored letters do not match the text color. This shows that people perform the perceptual task of reading words even when they have no incentive or desire to do so. This is likely because we become so good at reading over many years of training that the task becomes automatic and fewer attentional resources are needed to be devoted to it in order to read the words

26
Q

Gaming’s effects on the brain

A

Bavelier et al. (2012) found that non-gamers had to use their endogenous network to block out the irrelevant information. But for the gamers, blocking out irrelevant motion did not require much neural “effort”. Also, the activation of the area of the brain involved in motion processing was reduced for gamers. Gamers were better able to filter out the irrelevant motion stimulus and focus on the task at hand. Overall, these results suggest that the practice of allocating attention through extended gameplay increased the efficiency of these endogenous attention brain networks.

27
Q

Divided attention

A

The allocation of processing resources to multiple objects or tasks simultaneously. This is more commonly referred to as multi-tasking and, because there are so many things constantly vying for our attention, we often find ourselves trying to multi-task. Research has found that when we divide our attention among two tasks, performance on the individual tasks tends to suffer.

28
Q

Driving and phone calls

A

Drews et al. (2008) found a much larger number of driving errors for a cell-phone condition versus the number when talking to a passenger in a simulator. There appears to be something uniquely demanding about cell phone conversations since error rates are similarly higher for cell phone conversations compared to listening to the radio, or an audiobook. Also, evidence does not support the idea that talking hands-free using Bluetooth or some other technology is any safer than using a handset. This is because the problem is one of processing resources, not because you are using your hands for something other than holding the steering wheel.

29
Q

Can we actually pay attention to 2 tasks at once.

A

One theory is that what people are doing when they multitask is quickly switching their attention from one task to another. Task-switching has been shown to take time as the brain readjusts from the old task to the new task. Interestingly, people who multitask most often actually show the greatest deficits in task switching. Thus, someone who claims to be good at multi-tasking may actually be particularly bad at it.

30
Q

Evidence supporting the idea that we can multi-task

A

There is some evidence that people can pay attention to multiple objects concurrently. People can engage in multi-object tracking, keeping track of a set of targets as they move around on a screen among distractors. Research using multi-object tracking suggests that people can keep track of around four to five objects concurrently.

31
Q

The purpose of attention

A

One role of attention may be in terms of pre-activating, or readying, the processing needed for specific stimuli that are present or are about to be present. In this way, attention can make us more sensitive to stimuli and quicker to respond to them.

32
Q

Treisman’s feature-integration theory (1980s)

A

A theory of attentional function that holds that attention is necessary in order to bind together discrete features of an object unto a unified whole. Treisman found that when subjects were told to identify simple shapes or letters, their memory for objects would have predictable errors. For unattended stimuli, participants would sometimes only remember individual features correctly, not their conjunction. However, when participants were given meaning to the objects they were viewing, the attentional binding wasn’t necessary.

33
Q

Conjunction errors

A

A failure to accurately bind together the discrete features of a single object. Treisman theorized that they were due to the lack of attention binding them together. According to feature-integration theory, attention is needed in order to combine each of the properties into unified objects.

34
Q

Visual search paradigm

A

An experimental task in which participants must search for a target object among distractors. Treisman and others observed that when people have to search for a target that is different from distractors based on a single property, such as color or shape, the search was easy. Research shows that even if additional distractors are added to this kind of display, search times for the target do not get longer. The task becomes more difficult when the target is defined based on 2+ properties.

35
Q

Single-feature search

A

A version of a visual-search task in which the target is distinguished from the distractors based on a single feature.

36
Q

Conjunction search

A

A version of a visual-search task in which the target is distinguished from the distractors based on a several features. Experimental research on these kinds of displays shows that as the number of distractors increases from one simple feature to two, the amount of time it takes people to find the target increases linearly.

37
Q

Why does conjunction search take longer?

A

This is because it is necessary to attend to each stimulus in the array. As we attend to each stimulus, attentional processing is helping to bind the separate features (e.g., line orientation and color) in order to combine each item’s features to determine whether it is the target or not. This phenomenon suggests that a critical role of attention is to combine or integrate features into unified objects.

38
Q

Attention’s role in parallel processing

A

When there are several pieces of information to integrate in order to locate an object, attention may be operating across two channels and helping to complete parallel processing to more quickly discard items that are obviously not the target. However, attention’s role in guiding search functions and aiding perceptual processing cannot be simply reduced to acting as a binding agent or differentiated into serial and parallel processing models.

39
Q

Exogenous attentional control

A

Control of attention that is driven by factors external to the individual. The stimulus can draw attention to something automatically, whether we want to pay attention to it or not.

40
Q

Endogenous attentional control

A

Control of attention that is driven by factors internal to the individual. It occurs when a person chooses what to pay attention to, based on their goals or intentions (ex. choosing to focus on what you’re reading).

41
Q

Overt attention

A

Selective attention of a location that is accompanied by eye fixation of the same region. Looking directly at something is a way of maximizing the amount of visual information we can obtain about it.

42
Q

Covert attention

A

Attentional selection and processing of a location while eye fixation is maintained elsewhere.

43
Q

Why do we have both overt and covert attentions?

A

One possibility is that the direction of human gaze is easily detected by others and we don’t always want to reveal to others where we are attending. Another reason is that attention may sometimes precede eye movements by a short amount of time, as a way of scoping out the best place to move them.

44
Q

Attention in the brain

A

Certain forms of attention have been shown to be responsible for preparing relevant regions for activity (ex. occipital cortex). This form of attention that is based on anticipation and goal-directed appears to be a partially separable network than exogenous, bottom-up directed attention. In particular, it appears that regions in the posterior parietal lobes serve to control which of other brain areas are activated.

45
Q

Medial temporal lobes (MT)

A

A portion of the cortex that has been been established as a region that processed visual motion and near the occipital lobes, within the ventral stream of visual processing.

46
Q

fMRI study on attention (findings)

A

Participants had to report whether the dots were moving in the direction the arrow had pointed. fMRI showed that their MT was active. They also found enhanced activity around the intraparietal sulcus as well, even though it is not essential in processing motion. Instead, this suggests that the parietal lobe was active in modulating the activity of the medial temporal lobe. That is, the parietal lobe controls which portions of the brain are paying attention.

47
Q

Exogenours attention in the brain

A

There is evidence that regions localized to the right hemisphere, such as the temporoparietal junction, are responsible for capturing our attention to unexpected stimuli.

48
Q

Frontal Eye Fields (FEF)

A

A portion of the frontal lobes associated with allocation of attention via eye movements. This portion of the cortex is one area with a retinotopic map, which means that the spatial layout on the retina is preserved in the cortex, which in turn preserves the spatial layout of the visual world. The FEF appears to be engaged in the allocation of both overt and covert attention.

49
Q

Primary visual cortex

A

The portion of the occipital lobe that is the primary processing center for vision and organizes visual information for further processing throughout the occipital lobe. It also contains a retinotopic map.

50
Q

ADHD

A

Most commonly diagnosed in children and is associated with an inability to stay focused on a central task. Evidence supports that this distractibility is, at least in part, due to failures of the frontoparietal networks to control attention and suppress impulses.

51
Q

Go/no-go task

A

An experimental procedure used to test cognitive control and the subject’s ability to control impulsive responses. These tasks require a subject to press a button as quickly as possible when a target is present but stop pressing it when a different, but similar, target appears. The majority of stimuli are typically “go” stimuli, creating a situation where cognitive control is strongly needed to suppress the button-hitting response.

52
Q

Go/no-go task and ADHD

A

A study in 2003 found that children with ADHD completed more errors by responding to the no-go stimulus more often. Additionally, the researchers used fMRI to examine neural activity while participants responded, and found that control participants had a relatively increased level of neural activity in the frontoparietal regions when suppressing responses to the no-go stimuli. Participants with ADHD inefficiently activated these regions while responding to no-go trials. This demonstrates that there is a dysregulation in attention control networks that may increase levels of impulsive behaviors in ADHD patients.

53
Q

ADHD treatments

A

Symptoms of ADHD can be treated by behavioral management strategies that support both parent and child. Training programs can help provide parents with behavioral management strategies, as well as provide emotional support. Depending on the age of the child, training programs may be combined with medication (stimulants).

54
Q

Balint syndrome

A

A neurological disorder typically resulting from damage to both parietal lobes (ex. after a stroke) that carries several attentional deficits including occulomotor apraxia (the inability to execute visually guided movements) and simultanagnosia.

55
Q

Simultanagnosia

A

The inability to identify or use more than one object or property in a scene at a time. Paintings by Guiseppe Arcimboldo are sometimes used to test and demonstrate attentional deficits in patients with simultanagnosia. Patients with simultanagnosia focus on local features and struggle to see the global, holistic feature. While it can appear like some form of blindness, or even dementia, careful analysis of these patients reveals that it is attentional.

56
Q

Visual neglect

A

A deficit of attention in which the individual fails to notice or process a particular location in space, typically the left visual field due to right parietal lobe damage. Therefore, this is commonly referred to as left neglect syndrome. While it can appear that they are blind in that visual field, this is not the case. If an exogenous cue draws their attention to the left visual field, they will see and describe what is there. Instead, this is a deficit of attention: The damaged parietal lobe is causing the brain to ignore the left visual field.

57
Q

Left vs right parietal lobe and attention

A

Interestingly, damage to the left parietal lobe does not typically lead to neglect of the right visual field. This suggests that while the left parietal lobe only generates attention to the right visual field, the right parietal lobe can generate attention in both visual fields.

58
Q

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD)

A

A developmental disorder that can lead to deficits in language, impaired social behaviors, repetitive actions and general delays in cognitive development. ASD is generally thought of as being due to atypical brain development (although the cause is unknown).

59
Q

ASD and eye contact

A

People with ASD tend not to make eye contact. Research demonstrated altered visual tracking of facial expressions in ASD patients. Using eye tracking, the researchers compared the ability of individuals with and without autism to identify emotions being displayed by facial expressions and monitored where participants looked to make their decisions. Controls tended to look first at eyes and their visual paths concentrated around the center of the faces while those with autism avoided the eyes and center and instead tended to fixate outside regions of the face. It is possible that individuals with autism struggle to identify the emotions of others because they are not paying attention to the same information as controls.

60
Q

Why do people with ASD not engage in as much eye contact as others?

A

The origins of this tendency are not clear. It might be that people with autism have difficulty interpreting the mental states of others; this may make the eyes less useful as a site of fixation and so they fixate on other parts of the image. On the other hand, it may be that atypical fixations behaviors are part of autism from the beginning. hese abnormalities early on (as young as 6 months old) may contribute to the inability or the difficulty people with autism have of reading emotions and intentions on others’ faces.