Wijnen - Origin of NS Flashcards

(65 cards)

1
Q

What are crown eukaryotes, and which major groups are included in them?

A

Crown eukaryotes are the most evolved lineages of eukaryotes.

Include: plants, fungi, and animals.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Which groups represent the earliest diverging animals?

A

Sponges (Porifera)

Comb jellies (Ctenophores)

Comb jellies are thought to have diverged earlier than sponges.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Do sponges (Porifera) have a nervous system?

A

No, sponges do not have neurons or a nervous system.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Do comb jellies (Ctenophores) have a nervous system?

A

Yes, they have neurons, suggesting nervous systems may have evolved before the split from other animals.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Which groups show the emergence of a central nervous system and brain?

A

The central nervous system and brain emerge in bilaterians and cnidarians.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Are there nervous systems in unicellular organisms?

A

No

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What are the two main hypotheses about the origin of the nervous system based on comb jellies, sponges, and other animals?

A

Hypothesis 1: A nervous system evolved before comb jellies diverged, and was later lost in sponges.

Hypothesis 2: The nervous system in comb jellies evolved independently, separate from that in cnidarians and bilaterians.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Which simple animal group lacks a nervous system but is more closely related to humans than comb jellies or sponges?

A

Placozoans

Despite their simplicity and lack of a nervous system, they’re more closely related to cnidarians and bilaterians.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What behaviours do placozoans display despite lacking a nervous system?

A

They move (not sessile like sponges).

They can aggregate and forage.

Show coordinated movement, indicating some form of cell-cell communication.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

How do simple animals like sponges and placozoans move and feed without neurons?

A

Use cilia (hair-like structures) to move or generate water flow.

This enables basic movement and environmental interaction.

In more complex animals, muscles evolved to replace cilia for movement.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

What genetic evidence suggests placozoans may be related to animals with nervous systems?

A

Genomic analysis shows they possess some genes shared with nervous systems, even though they lack neurons.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What does the behaviour of placozoans suggest about early nervous system evolution?

A

Complex behaviour can occur without neurons.

Implies cell-cell communication evolved before the nervous system.

Raises questions about how coordination and movement were managed pre-neuronally.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What are Myxozoa and why are they surprising from an evolutionary perspective?

A

Myxozoa are unicellular animals.

They are highly derived cnidarians that lost their nervous system.

Initially mistaken for protists due to their size and simplicity.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

How was it discovered that Myxozoa are animals?

A

Genomic sequencing revealed their evolutionary lineage.

Despite their simple, unicellular appearance, their genes link them to cnidarians.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Why did Myxozoa lose their nervous system?

A

They evolved a parasitic lifestyle, relying on hosts.

This allowed them to shed complex functions like having neurons.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

What is the life cycle of Myxozoa like?

A

Involves two hosts: a fish and an annelid worm.

They undergo both sexual and asexual reproduction.

Produce spores during their cycle.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Where do Myxozoa fall within the animal phylogenetic tree?

A

Genetically, they sit within Cnidaria, between groups like sea anemones and jellyfish.

Indicates that they once had neurons but lost them secondarily.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Can unicellular organisms sense and respond to their environment?

A

Yes, they can sense, integrate, and respond to environmental cues.

They exhibit behaviour without a nervous system.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Why don’t unicellular organisms need a nervous system?

A

Everything—sensing, integration, and response—happens within one cell.

No need for a circuit of multiple cells like in a nervous system.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Do unicellular organisms have precursors to neuronal signalling components?

A

Yes, they have molecular precursors (e.g. ion channels, signalling proteins) similar to those used in neurons.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

How is the basic function of a neuron similar to a unicellular organism?

A

Both sense their environment, process input, and generate output (behaviour/action).

The key difference: neurons are part of circuits, unicellular organisms operate independently.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

What is chemotaxis and which organisms exhibit it?

A

Chemotaxis is movement towards an attractant or away from a repellent chemical gradient.

Exhibited by both bacteria (e.g. E. coli) and animals (e.g. C. elegans).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

How can we experimentally show that chemotaxis is directed behaviour?

A

Measure the fraction of movement “runs” that are oriented toward an attractant over time.

Longer runs are biased toward attractants, indicating purposeful behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

What example illustrates bacterial chemotaxis?

A

E. coli moves toward aspartate using biased random walks.

Despite being unicellular, it shows goal-directed behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
What example illustrates animal chemotaxis?
C. elegans moves toward salt gradients using its nervous system. Behaviourally similar to E. coli, but with neuronal control.
26
Do you need a nervous system to perform chemotaxis?
No — unicellular organisms like E. coli can perform chemotaxis. But a nervous system helps coordinate and refine this behaviour in multicellular organisms.
27
What are the three main components of bacterial chemotaxis systems?
Sensory system: detects attractants/repellents. Signal integration system: uses kinases and methylation mechanisms. Motor system: controls flagella for movement.
28
What protein acts as the adaptor in bacterial chemotaxis and what does it connect?
The CheW adaptor links sensory receptors to the CheA histidine kinase.
29
How is the chemotaxis signal transmitted to the flagella in bacteria?
CheA kinase activates CheY, the response regulator. Phosphorylated CheY interacts with the flagellar motor, changing rotation.
30
What roles do CheR and CheB play in bacterial chemotaxis?
CheR (methyltransferase): adds methyl groups to receptors, adapting to steady signals. CheB (methylesterase): removes methyl groups, modulating receptor sensitivity.
31
What is the purpose of adaptation in bacterial chemotaxis?
Ensures bacteria respond to changes in attractant concentration, not constant presence. Allows them to maintain sensitivity to gradients and avoid saturation.
32
How does phosphatase activity contribute to bacterial chemotaxis?
Phosphatases reset the system by dephosphorylating response regulators like CheY. Prevents continuous activation, enabling gradient tracking.
33
Why is bacterial chemotaxis considered less precise than animal navigation?
Bacteria move using biased random walks, not direct paths. Animals (with nervous systems) can navigate more directly and rapidly.
34
What are the structural features of a voltage-gated sodium channel?
24 transmembrane domains Voltage sensor, inactivation gate, selectivity filter Forms a regulated pore for Na⁺ during action potentials.
35
How did voltage-gated sodium channels evolve?
Evolved from voltage-gated calcium channels (which also have 24 transmembrane domains). These, in turn, arose via duplication of 12-transmembrane channels, which evolved from 6-transmembrane channels.
36
What evolutionary pattern is seen in ion channel development?
Gene duplication and domain repetition enabled complexity. For example: - 6-transmembrane → 12-transmembrane → 24-transmembrane - Simple potassium channels show early evolutionary stages.
37
Why is a single large ion channel gene with repeats more evolutionarily flexible?
Mutations can affect individual subunits without changing all of them. Allows functional divergence (e.g. evolving a voltage sensor in one domain only).
38
Where are simple ion channels (2 or 6 transmembrane) found, and what does this tell us?
Found in prokaryotes. Indicates ion channels evolved before the nervous system and even before eukaryotes.
39
What does the evolutionary history of ion channels suggest about nervous systems?
Nervous system components (e.g. ion channels) predate neurons. Suggests neurons reused ancient molecular tools for new signalling roles.
40
Do synaptic components originate only in animals?
No — many components predate animals. Some are shared with choanoflagellates, sponges, and even plants and amoebae.
41
What does the colour coding in this diagram of synapse components represent?
Orange: Components shared with choanoflagellates (unicellular relatives of animals) Yellow: Components shared with cnidarians and placozoans Blue: Components shared with sponges Green: Components shared with plants, amoebae, or ciliates
42
What does the wide distribution of synaptic components tell us about nervous system evolution?
Synaptic signalling repurposes ancient cellular machinery. Most synaptic proteins evolved from pre-existing cell communication systems used before neurons existed.
43
Are any synaptic components unique to bilaterians?
Very few components are unique to bilaterians. Most are found in more basal animal groups or unicellular relatives.
44
What are some conserved brain structures across vertebrates?
Cerebrum Optic tectum Cerebellum Olfactory bulb
45
How do vertebrate brains differ despite shared structures?
The size and organisation of brain regions vary by species. Reflects sensory and behavioural specialisations.
46
What genes are responsible for patterning animal body axes?
Homeobox (Hox) transcription factors Clustered in the genome and direct body axis development.
47
How do Hox gene clusters vary between animal groups?
Deuterostomes (e.g. humans) have multiple paralogs of Hox genes. Protostomes (e.g. Drosophila) have fewer but similar Hox genes. Cnidarians already express Hox-like genes for oral–aboral patterning.
48
What does the conservation of Hox genes suggest about animal evolution?
Body patterning mechanisms are deeply conserved across animal phyla. Suggests a common ancestral toolkit for axis formation.
49
How is the nervous system patterned in animals like flies and frogs?
Patterned by morphogen gradients (e.g. Dpp in flies, BMP4 in frogs). These gradients regulate dorsoventral axis and neural development.
50
What is unusual about the body plan comparison between vertebrates and arthropods?
Vertebrates (e.g. frogs, humans): nervous system is dorsal. Arthropods (e.g. flies): nervous system is ventral. Suggests an inverted body plan, possibly due to early divergence.
51
Are transcriptional regulators for nervous system patterning conserved?
Yes — similar transcription factors pattern neural domains in both flies and vertebrates. Indicates a shared ancestral mechanism, despite inverted body axis.
52
Why is studying nervous system development in simpler animals useful?
Many molecular mechanisms are conserved, making model organisms like Drosophila or C. elegans useful for understanding neuronal function.
53
Why can we study nervous system function using model organisms?
The majority of human genes are shared with non-vertebrate animals. Many aspects of neuronal structure, function, and signalling are conserved.
54
Which model organisms are commonly used to study the nervous system?
Rodents (e.g. mice, rats) Invertebrates (e.g. Drosophila, C. elegans) Yeast (e.g. budding yeast) — for studying mitochondrial aspects of diseases like Parkinson’s
55
How many neurons does the human brain have, and how many synaptic connections?
~86 billion neurons Over 100 trillion synaptic connections
56
Why is it difficult to study human brains experimentally?
Long generation time (~20–30 years) Ethical and practical limits on manipulation High complexity: 24,000 genes and immense neural connectivity
57
How many neurons are in the rat brain?
About 200 million neurons
58
What is the advantage of using rats or mice in neuroscience?
Shorter lifespans, more accessible to manipulation and breeding Simpler but still complex nervous systems suitable for study
59
How many neurons does the fruit fly (Drosophila) have, and what is notable about it?
~100,000 neurons Entire fly brain has been mapped (connectome completed) Allows for detailed study of circuit function and behaviour
60
How many genes do flies have and how is their genome organised?
~15,000 genes Typically single versions of genes (not paralogues like in humans)
61
What makes C. elegans a powerful model in neuroscience?
Only ~1,000 cells, with 302 neurons Neurons and connections are identical in every worm Connectome of ~7,000 synapses is fully mapped
62
What does it mean that C. elegans has a predetermined nervous system?
No plasticity — every neuron and connection is fixed and identical Allows for predictable, stereotyped behaviours
63
How does neuronal plasticity differ between humans, flies, and worms?
Humans: highly plastic, new connections formed constantly Flies: show some plasticity C. elegans: no plasticity, fully deterministic wiring
64
Why can’t the human nervous system be fully hardwired like in C. elegans?
The complexity of the human brain exceeds what can be genetically preprogrammed Plasticity allows for adaptability, learning, and higher cognition
65