Adaption Flashcards
(85 cards)
Adaptation
- describes the process by which the visual system alters its operating properties in response to recent changes in the environment
- widespread property of neural sensory systems
- effects range from influencing response of individual cells in the visual brain to shaping our perception of the world
always happening, passive process
luminance of a retinal image depends on 2 factors
- surface illumination
- relative surface reflectance
surface illumination
the amount of light falling onto an object
relative surface reflectance
(albedo)
the proportion of light that is reflected back from the surface
how much light is absorbed & how much is reflected to our eyes
dynamic range of light intensity
the luminance of a pieve of white paper is 1b times higher in outdoor sunlight than on a moonless night
output range of the retina
- the output cells of the retina (RGC) have a limited response range - ~1-300 spikes per second
- the retina must try to:
- accomodate large changes in mean luminance
- maintain sensitivity to differences in luminance within a scene
pupil changes
- the overall amount of light entering the eye is regulated by controlling the size of the circular pupil
- pupillary light reflex
- but in humans this accounts for only a 10-fold to 20-fold change in the intensity of light on the retina - still 8 orders of magnitude
- vertical slit pupils found in other species are slightly more effective e.g. cats
pupillary light reflex
high intensity light causes the pupil causes the pupil to become smaller, whereas low intensity light causes the pupil to become larger
lets us change how much light comes in
duplex function
- the operating range of the retina is increased by having distinct receptor systems which are specialised for different lighting conditions
- scotopic vision = low light conditions, rods
- photopic vision = well lit conditions, cones
overlap = mesopic vision, use both rods and cones
rods
duplex function
- highly sensitive (can respond to a single photon of light)
- high convergence (average of 120 rods to one RGC)
- saturate under daylight conditions
cones
duplex function
- less light sensitive (requires 100s of photons to respond)
- low convergence (average of 6 cones to one RGC, but one-to-one relationship in fovea)
- continue to respond in high light conditions
dark adaptation
- what happens if we suddenly shift from a well lit area to a darkened room?
- at first it is difficult to see anything, but over time you adjust to the lower light conditions around it becomes easier to see your way around
measuring dark adaptation
- expose subject to a uniform adapting light source
- sit them in a darkened room
- flash a very dim light with increasing intensity until the subject can just detect it
- repeat this process at regular time intervals
- plot the subject’s light detection threshold as a function of time in the dark
trying to find threshold sensitivity
viewing conditions for a dark adaptation experiment
the image of the fixation point falls on the fovea and the image of the test light falls in the peripheral retina
- present off to the side
- stimulate the part of the retina that has both rods & cone photoreceptors
key features of dark adaptation
- increase in sensitivity over time
- two distinct branches, due to the transition from cones to rods
- within each branch there is significant adaptation
- cone sensitivity adjustment takes 4-5 mins
- rod sensitivity adjustment takes 20-30 mins
bleaching/regeneration of photopigment
- after a photopigment molecule is bleached (used to detect a photon of light), the molecule must be regenerated before it can be used again
- in low light levels, more rod and cone photopigments are gradually regenerated, increasing their ability to respond to photons of light
- recovery of rod photopigment (rhodopsin) is slower than the 3 types of cone photopigments
- concentration of photopigments (how many are available at a given time)
- rhodopsin = rod photopigment
- bleaching - retinal and opsin break apart
- how it turns into neural response
- increasing time = greater concentration which means we are more sensitive to light
negative feedback
- feedback from horizontal cells onto the photoreceptors also changes their sensitivity
- if horizontal cells respond strongly they tell the photoreceptors to turn it down
- horizontal cells give inhibitory feedback
- in-built sensitivity check with retina
light adaptation
- incoming light bleaches the plentiful rod and cone photopigments, leading to a massive firing in the RGCs
- at first everything seems glaringly bright and ‘washed out’, but things return to normal after a minute or so
- reverse to dark adaptation: pupil constricts to reduce light input, shift to photopic system, reduction in photoreceptor sensitivity
light and dark adaptation in real world
- why some fighter pilots wear red glasses before a night flight - rods are not sensitive to red light, making it possible to dark adapt prior to entering dark conditions
- why star-gazers use red torches - avoid bleaching rod system once dark adapted
red goggles block out all light except long wavelengths
perceptual consequences of light/dark adaptation
lightness constancy
- white paper inside a room reflects less light than black paper does outside
- perception of lightness doesnt simply reflect the absolute luminance of an object
- luminance constrast - the difference between an object’s luminance and the background
- in the photopic range, the entire operating range of RGCs shifts according to mean light level
- known as gain control & results in responses that represent contrast rather than absolute intensity
- gain control in retina ensures that the visual system maintains sensitivity to contrast despite large changes in mean luminance
- weber’s law & just discriminable difference
- when changing the sensitivity of the retina, proportions will stay the same
- respond to percentage rather than absolute light level
- gain control: matching output to input
consequences of light/dark adaptation
negative afterimages
- complementary coloured aftereffects occur due to the opponent nature of chromatic pathways
- e.g. adaptation to red causes a reduction in the sensitvity of long wavelength cones, creating an imbalance in the inputs to red/green opponent RGCs
- Van Lier et al. (2009) - some form of ‘filling-in- of the afterimage across shape contour
- byproduct of shift in sensitivity
- using colour opponency and sensitivities in retina
perceptual consequences of light/dark adaptation
troxler fading
- under steady fixation, stationary objects in the periphery tend to fade and disappear
- movement of the eyes immediately restores visibility
- torxler fading only occurs in peripheral vision, most likely because receptive fields in the periphery are considerably larger than our fixational eye movements
- faint stimulus in periphery
- lilac chase = troxler fading and negative afterimaging
V1 neural response properties
- small receptive fields
- orientation and spatial frequency tuning
- contrast-dependent response
- binocularity
- small receptive fields: Only respond to visual input from small area of space. excitatory & inhibitory regions
- areas are elongated
- contrast-dependent response. diffs between darkest & lightest regions in space
- binocularity: information from two eyes get inputted into one another
contrast adaptation in V1
repeated presentation of a preferred stimulus reduces neural responsivity
- over time: reduction in responsivity
- how well they responded as a function of the contrasts
- bursting pattern corresponding to graphs
- reduce contrast: less and less response