lecture 2- behavioural nudging Flashcards
(50 cards)
what makes nudging powerful?
- randomised controlled trails (RCTs)
- A/B testing
social influence
- Six principles of influence:
1. Reciprocation
2. Social proof
3. Liking
4. Authority
5. Commitment & self-consistency
6. Scarcity - Related terms:
Persuasion, Conformity, Compliance, Obedience
why is reciprocation so powerful?
- No human society that doesn’t
reciprocate (Gouldner, 1960) - Allows us to give and not lose
- Allows division of labour,
trading, expertise, efficient
social groups
reciprocation- cont
- Compliance experiment Regan 1971 JESP
- supposedly about “art appreciation”
- a confederate “bought” the participant a Coke (or not)
- At the end, the confederate asked participants to buy
raffle tickets so he could win a competition. - 2x tickets bought in Coke condition compared to
baseline
reciprocation
- Compliance experiment Regan 1971 JESP
- In 1971, a coke cost 10 cents …. but the average
number of tickets bought in Coke condition was 2
tickets, which was worth 50 cents. - 400% return on investment!
reciprocation- powerful
- Very powerful
- people gave more money to the confederate when they liked him better
(confederate either behaved nice or mean to third person) - BUT only when he didn’t buy them a coke Regan 1971 JESP
- when he bought them a coke, people reciprocated regardless of liking
- Note that the gift giver chooses
both the gift and the favour - principle behind free samples:
reciprocation: enforcing
- Enforces uninvited debt
- people give even when they don’t want to (e.g. no one turned down
the free Coke) - makes sense, if rule evolved to help maximise group cohesion i.e.
people can give without loss
Charities use this
technique:
3x more donations
British Red Cross, reported in the
Guardian, 2014
- Reciprocal concessions
- Door-in-Face technique: obligation to make a concession to
someone who has made a concession to you Cialdini et al 1975, JPSP
3x as many people agree
to chaperone juvenile
delinquents to the zoo
after declining larger
favour Cialdini et al 1975, JPSP
social proof: the big mistake
- Injunctive norms (what we’re supposed to do)
- Descriptive norms (what most people do)
- Study looked at people removing petrified wood from a National Park in
Arizona Cialdini et al 2006, Social Influence - Compared the effectiveness of different messages set along visitor paths
- Dropped petrified wood along the paths (to measure how much wood was
taken by visitors)
social proof: the big mistake
- Injunctive norms (what we’re supposed to do)
- Descriptive norms (what most people do)
- “Many past visitors have removed petrified wood” (Negative) descriptive
norm Cialdini et al 2006, Social Influence - “Please don’t remove the petrified wood from the Park, in order to
preserve the natural state of the Petrified Forest” (Positive) injunctive
norm
-> 8% wood removed
-> 1.7% wood removed
Social proof: When and why?
- When and why do we follow the group?
- Similarity
- We follow people like us:
- in the tax study, “people in the same town” was more effective than
“people in the UK” - in the towel study, “same room” was more effective than “same hotel”
social proof: when and why?
- When and why do we follow the group?
- When uncertain (informational social influence)
- e.g. bystander effect, Kitty Genovese murder (35 mins and 38 people who
didn’t do anything) - pluralistic ignorance -
“smoke” experiment Latene & Darley 1968, JPSP - 75% people reported the smoke
when on own - 10%, when confederates didn’t move
liking: why do we like others
- physical attractiveness
- similarity
- cooperation and synchrony
- compliments
- personalisation
Liking: physical attractiveness
Beauty premium:
* more attractive people are paid around 5 to
10 percent more Hameresh & Biddle 1993, Beauty and the Labor Market
* … are viewed as being more socially
competent Eagly et al Psych Bulletin 1991
* … even receive lighter sentences in the
criminal justice system Stewart, J. Applied Psychology 1980
- Beautiful people are more
persuasive Chaiken JPSP, 1974 - Supports associative learning
(anything associated with beauty
is also perceived as good) - brands exploit the halo effect to
sell products
similarity
- We like people more when they are
similar to us - “Hippies” study: people asked for a
favour (borrow a dime for a payphone)
by confederates dressed either as
“Hippies” or “Straights” Emswiller et al 1971
Journal of Applied Social Psych. - Favour was granted more often when
confederates’ clothes matched the
participants’
- We like people more when they are
similar to us - “Hippie” experimenter got more
signatures on a peace petitions at a real
peace rally than “Straight” experimenter
Suedfeld et al 1971 Journal of Applied Social Psych. - Also, more people signed in the Hippie
condition without looking at the petition!
Liking: cooperation and synchrony
- “Robbers’ Cave” experiment: when the Eagles and Rattlers
were competing over camp resources, hostilities rapidly
increased. Increasing cooperation between the groups
increased liking and in turn, further cooperation Sherif & Sherif 1954 - Mimicry increases liking Chatrand & Bargh 1999, JPSP
- Synchrony in action increases liking Hove & Risen 2009 Social Cognition
- Synchrony increases helping behaviours: participants and
confederate asked to tap along with music (on headphones):
Valdesolo & DeSteno 2011, Emotion - after unsynchronised tapping (different music), 18% of
participants stayed to help confederate with maths tasks - after synchronised tapping (same music), 49% stayed to help
- likelihood of helping was mediated by sense of similarity
comliments
Liking: compliments
* Robust effect of compliments on liking
* Surprisingly little research on the effect
of compliments on persuasion
* “Tips” studies: compliments lead to
higher tips Seiter 2007 J Applied Social Psychology
‘‘Flattery will get you everywhere’’
Mae West
being personal
- In general, remembering details about a person is perceived as
an index of how important you think they are Ray et al JPSP 2019 - Remembering names is perceived as a compliment Howard et al 1995
J. Consumer Research - Remembering someone’s name also increases the likelihood that
the person will make a purchase, mediated by the compliment
value Howard et al 1995 J. Consumer Researc
Criticisms of experiments
- Demand bias? (participants guess hypothesis)
- Mitigated by:
- making sure participants are blind to the experiment
hypotheses and/or the condition - asking at the end what the experiment was
about (funnel debrief) - use between-subjects design
e.g. synchrony studies Hove & Risen 2009 Social Psychology
Criticisms of experiments
- Experimenter bias? (most times, the experimenters knew the
hypothesis and so could have acted differently) - Mitigated with:
- strict decision criteria for choosing a participant (e.g. Hippie study used
specific times, approach first person who meets criteria: Emswiller et al 1971 Journal of Applied Social
Psych.) - making experimenters blind
- record interactions
- use computer interaction (so completely
scripted) e.g. Grant et al 2010 Basic and Applied Social Psychology
Authority
- Milgram experiments: people stopped shocks if…
… the experimenter told the participant to stop
(even if the “victim” insisted they continue)
… the experimenter and “victim” switched roles
… with two experimenters who disagreed - doctors study: researchers phoned wards and pretended to be doctors
and asked the nurses to administer a dangerous dose of unauthorised
medication to a specific patient Hofling et al J. of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1966 - in 95% of cases, the nurses went to the medicine cabinet and secured
the medicine (where they were stopped!)
authority
- Dominance v. credibility
- Credibility: expertise and trustworthiness