Lecture 21 - Schemas and Scripts Flashcards

1
Q

Schemas

scripts?

A

knowledge about the world

− Scripts are a specific type of schema: sequence of events in schema

structure in memory

pre-loaded experiences telling you how to encode current info: shapes interpretation of what’s happening now and how you recall that info later

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

So far, out study of memory (STM/WM and LTM) has focused
on lists or words or simple sentences. Is this what memory is
typically used for?

A

• Outside the laboratory, our memory encodes current information
to help us understand situations and act within them.

• Memories of similar/relevant instances in the past reduce the
cognitive complexity of the present situation.

• Memory is a constructive process, using current input + topdown
(previously encoded) knowledge.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Outside the laboratory, our memory encodes…

A

…current information

to help us understand situations and act within them.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Memories of similar/relevant instances in the past reduce the…

A

…cognitive complexity of the present situation.

it simplifies the present moment because some of the things that have been experienced before don’t need to be encoded

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Memory is a constructive process,

A

using current input mixed with topdown (previously encoded) knowledge.

all memories being dynamically mixed with what you’re experiencing now

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

A study by Anderson et al (1977) used the same text,

interpreting the same information in different ways

A

took different populations of students instead of priming them with images like we did in class

• 64% of physical education majors interpreted the passage as having to do with wrestling.

• Only 28% of music majors thought it was associated with
wrestling.

• Default: Most interpreted it as a prisoner planning escape.

suggests: you bring a certain background anytime you’re interpreting new information - but if someone is primed with an image that interpretation can be altered

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

recall

A

is a reconstructive process

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q
In a similar study by Bransford and Johnson (1972) using
passages of complex text describing a scene, recall for
events in the story doubled (32% vs 16%) when a
descriptive title ("the doctor" or the dentist") was provided.
A

just enough to activate top-down info to shape how you recall

able to frame things, encode them within this schema

Clearly some semantic priming is occurring, but how does
it work for a complex event?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

A schema is….

A

…..a general knowledge structure used for
understanding or interpreting complex situations (information).
- any real world setting

something you do repeatedly you will build a schema for that event (dinner, lecture, travel, ect…)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

qualities of schemas

A

• A schema is general for a type of situation, and helps give organizational structure to an specific event.
- helps you structure individual episodes

• Schemas are more than a bunch of facts – they are structured to include the relationships between facts.

• Most importantly, schemas provide relevant top-down knowledge.
- what you bring to bear in any new situation

• For example, you might have schemas for what to expect and what to do at the movies, a basketball game, lecture, family
dinner, etc.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

A schema is general for a type of situation, and helps give

A

organizational structure to an specific event.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Based on an accumulation of events, you build up schemas

for different situations. This builds….

A

…. expectations for specific elements during some episode.

because you know what to expect you don’t have to do as much detection

e.g. The last time you went out to a restaurant:

Typical ‘dining’ schema elements:
• What kind of food do they serve?
• What did you eat?
• Was it good?
• Was it expensive?
• Was the service good?

Less typical ‘dining’ schema elements:

  • Did the server write down your order?
  • Were any children present?
  • Were there any clowns?

any schema will be general for a specific situation and will have typical and nontypical elements

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

slots

values

default assumption

A

Each schema has slots that are placeholders for expected (typical) items. Each slot can be filled with (appropriate) values from the current situation. If no value for a slot is
provided, the schema may have a default assumption.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Restaurant dining slots

EX

A
  • Food types served:
  • Food purchased:
  • Food quality:
  • Cost:
  • Service quality:

more declarative stuff

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Your individual defaults

EX

A
 Italian
 Chicken parmesan
 Fair
 Cheap
 Just okay
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Current values

EX

A
 Italian
 Ravioli
 Good
 Cheap
 ? - fill in default value of "just okay"
17
Q

default value

A

When some information is
incomplete or isn’t encoded, you
can assume the default value.

are both what you expect to happen and what is pre-built into the schema

episodes over time build up your defaults

you fill in default values when you don’t remember

they tend to be more individual related

18
Q

Schemas are more than a bunch of facts –

A

they are structured

to include the relationships between facts.

19
Q

Most importantly, schemas provide

A

relevant top-down

knowledge.

20
Q

example schemas

A

you might have schemas for what to expect and

what to do at the movies, a basketball game, lecture, family dinner, etc.

21
Q

The default values in schemas can lead to characteristic mistakes.

Brewer and Treyens (1981) examined schemas for places.
- doctor office, dentist office, library, etc… (you expect certain things there)

A
  • Participants waited in a graduate student office for 35 seconds.
  • They were then taken out and asked to recall the items in the office.

Subjects had good recall for items that fit a graduate office schema
(i.e. desk, chair). However, 30% of
subjects reported seeing books, even though none were present.

==> Books are a default expectation, so they are recalled even though
they were not present in that instance.

22
Q

People in the same cultures…

A

…tend to share the same slots in schemas, but your individual defaults may vary based on personal experience and preferences.

23
Q

the schema is your…

A

framework for interpreting things and you have certain expectations and defaults

24
Q

People in the same cultures tend to share the same slots in
schemas, but your individual defaults may vary based on
personal experience and preferences.

ex?

A
  • People in the U.S. tend to tip after a meal, but the default amount may vary.
  • People in different cultures may not tip at all.

• You can also embed schemas. For example, the going-to-dinner
schema could be part of a going-on-a-date schema.

25
Q

A script is a…

A

… specific type of schema. They are much more
structured and have a specific order of events.

 * lots of scripts often embedded in a schema.
 * There are causal links between events in the script. Changing an event (outcome) changes the script.
26
Q

script or schema?

what happens if you change the outcome of the slot?

A

Food purchased: (?)

Script slots
• Order is placed: True
• Food arrives: True
• Order is correct: False
• Pay for food: False
27
Q

Do people actually use scripts? Bower and colleagues (1972)
did a number of experiments regarding how people produce and
use scripts.

Experiment 1

A

• Participants were asked to free-associate and write a list of actions
that they think people do in various situations (e.g. go to a lecture, get up in the morning, go to a restaurant, etc.).

• They found great uniformity for “basic actions” described in scripts (e.g. out of 730 total actions (total slots to be filled by schemas) for ‘restaurant’ only four were completely unique and were given by a single person).

• This suggests that people encode and later recall similar elements
from a similar type of event.

28
Q

Bower and colleagues (1972) also asked (different)

participants to recall facts about stories.

A

• People were presented with passages that contained six ordered events/actions. They each had a title (e.g. “The
Doctor).

   − Stories could be in typical order (typical script) or scrambled.
  • They were later (after a 20 minute delay) provided with just the title and asked to recall each line from the story.
  • Stories presented in the correct order were recalled with 50%

accuracy. Stories in scrambled order can 18% accuracy.
- causal relationship made recall easier: logical: easier to encode, easier to recall

  • Recall was better when the items followed the typical script, suggesting an organization role in understanding and memory.
  • Additionally, people added 1 fact (on average) that was plausible given the context.
29
Q

Schemas and scripts seem most compatible with which

model of memory?

A

PDP model: encoding context at the same time that we’re encoding declarative facts
- we make up a story that fits the high points

The descriptions of schemas and scripts seem very vague
and hard to characterize.

30
Q

Although schemas seem to be made of _____, much of the activity appears more ____ and harder to state (confabulate).

A

declarative facts

procedural

The rules and dependencies seem like post hoc confabulation

have this memory system to encode or recall or a system to make up stories (rules for confabulation)

because we’re doing stuff in a scene and then we have to state later

31
Q

How are the dependencies established, particularly when

slots can be filled with new items?

A

PDP may be a way of addressing this, as experience
forms the dependency

use episodic info to build semantic info

but no two situations are exactly the same - details always in flux

32
Q

Schemas and scripts should break down when

A

some defaults are violated.

33
Q

The descriptions of schemas and scripts seem very vague

and hard to characterize.

A
  • describe things in terms what you’ve seen before: what’s your memory? stuff that happens a lot: circular: just remembering those things that have happened before

because things are responding largely to the context

34
Q

Just as we use schemas to understand (encode) events, we also use them later during recall.

A

• Source monitoring is the process of determining the origin of
some memory or belief.
• Sometimes we use stereotypes (a schema for a person) to
reconstruct a memory.
• This can be very useful – as with all schemas you don’t need to
recall every detail, but can fill-in with defaults.
• However, this can (and does) lead to many errors.

35
Q

Source monitoring

A

is the process of determining the origin of
some memory or belief.

trying to decide: where did that memory come from?

what is the original source for the thing currently in working memory?

36
Q

Sometimes we use stereotypes (a schema for a person) to

A

reconstruct a memory.

reduces computational complexity: know what to expect

don’t have to pay as much attention: you know what to expect

fill in with default values

37
Q

if you build up episodes and build up schemas to deal with situations and people

A

it takes a lot of new episodes to change

38
Q

Marsh, Cook, & Hicks (2006) examined the

role of gender stereotypes on source (what produced the memory) attributions.

A

• Subjects read statements (e.g. “I like
baseball.”), that were attributed to one of two
people: Chris or Pat.
• Sentences were masculine, feminine, or
neutral.
• Subjects were given a puzzle (delay) and later
told the gender of the people (Chris is a
heterosexual male, Pat is a heterosexual
female).
• They then re-read the statements, and asked
the source (who had said them).
• People were more accurate when the
statements matched the stereotypes
(schemas).