Maternal Deprivation Hypothesis and PDD Flashcards

1
Q

What are possible causes of disruption in attachment?

A

Parent’s job occupation.

Presence of grandparents.

Presence of carers e.g. at nursery.

Abuse.

Health issues.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Who created the maternal deprivation hypothesis? When?

A

Bowlby, 1951.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What does the MDH explain?

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

What happens when these monotropic attachments are broken.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

What did Bowlby believe happened if the monotropy is disrupted?

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

Bowlby argues that disruption of the attachment results in serious and permanent damage to a child’s emotional, social and intellectual development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What did Bowlby believe was the best way to examine the validity of the MDH?

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

To assess the effects of disruption to an infants’ attachments.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Why was Bowlby’s MDH revolutionary?

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

At the time most professionals assumed meeting physical needs were most important for development e.g. minerals and vitamins.

In this case, Bowlby introduced the idea that psychological needs must also be met - and that this is important for future relationships, cognitive skills and social development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

According to the MDH, disruption can occur in three basic ways, what are they?

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

Short-term separation.

Long-term deprivation.

Privation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What is separation? Give an example.

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

Short term disruption.

Involves distress when separated from a person to whom there is an attachment for a relatively short period of time.

A mild form of deprivation.

E.g. parent job occupation, health issues, holidays, etc.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What is deprivation? Give an example.

(Bowlby’s MDH).

A

Long term disruption.

It occurs when an attachment has been formed and is then broken for what is generally fairly long periods of time.

E.g. health issues, parent job occupation e.g. military, etc.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Who proposed the PDD model? When?

(Short-term separation).

A

Robertson and Bowlby, 1952.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

What did Robertson and Bowlby do in 1952?

(Short-term separation).

A

Studied young children separated from their mothers for some time (hospital).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Robertson and Bowlby were among the first to study what in detail?

(Short-term separation).

A

The effects of short-term separation in young children.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What did Robertson and Bowlby identify in 1952?

(Short-term separation).

A

Clearly identified the main stages in children’s response to separation.

Highlighting the importance of minimising the adverse effects of separation on young children.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

What are the 3 stages in the PDD model?

(Short-term separation).

A

Protest.

Despair.

Detachment.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What does the PDD model explain?

(Short-term separation).

A

A child’s response to separation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

What is short-term separation?

(Short-term separation).

A

Consists of brief, temporary separations from attachment figures, like attending day care, being left with a baby sitter, or a short period of hospitalisation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Who created the stages in the PDD model? When?

(Short-term separation).

A

Bowlby, 1969.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

What did Bowlby do in 1969?

(Short-term separation).

A

Described the distress (effects) caused by short-term separation in terms of the PDD model.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

What are the 3 factors in the PDD model?

(Short-term separation).

A

Protest.

Despair.

Detachment.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Outline ‘protest’ as a factor in the PDD model.

(Short-term separation).

A

The child cries, screams and protests angrily when the parent leaves.

They will try to cling on to the parent to stop them leaving.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Outline ‘despair’ as a factor in the PDD model.

(Short-term separation).

A

The child’s protest begins to stop, and they appear to be calmer although still upset.

The child refuses others’ attempts for comfort and often seems withdrawn and uninterested in anything.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Outline ‘detachment’ as a factor in the PDD model.

(Short-term separation).

A

During which the child seems to behave in a less distressed way.

If the mother of caregiver re-appears during this stage, they are not responded to with any great interest and may be rejected.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Outline Douglas’ research from 1975.

(Short-term separation).
(Supports MDH).

A

Supports Bowlby’s MDH.

This is because they found separations of less than a week for children below the age of 4 correlate with behavioural difficulties.

This suggests that even short disruptions to early attachments can be detrimental to infant development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Outline Robertson and Robertson’s research from 1971.

(Short-term separation).
(Challenges PDD and MDH).

A

Challenges the PDD model, as well as the MDH.

This is because they found that short-term separation does not have to produce negative effects if the following factors were implemented:

Children introduced to their new surroundings beforehand.
Provide children with a similar routine.
Discuss their mother with them, allows for a reduction in stress.
Good physical and emotional care from the PCG substitute.

This suggests that the PDD model can be avoided and attachment breakage can be minimised, and is not a universal feature of monotropic attachment breakdown.

25
Q

What did Bowlby say about the effects of deprivation on an infant?

(Long-term deprivation)

A

They were negative and permanent/ irreversible.

26
Q

What is long-term deprivation?

(Long-term deprivation)

A

Involves lengthy or permanent separations from attachment figures, most commonly due to divorce.

27
Q

What percentage of UK marriages end in divorce?

(Long-term deprivation)

A

42%.

28
Q

Within two to three years of divorce, what percentage of divorced parents not living with their children (usually the father) had lost contact with their children?

(Long-term deprivation)

A

50%.

29
Q

Give an example of long-term deprivation.

(Long-term deprivation)

A

E.g. death or imprisonment of a parent and resulting adoption by different caregivers.

30
Q

What is privation?

(Privation)

A

Concerns children who have never formed an attachment bond.

31
Q

Is privation or deprivation more detrimental to an infants development? What do research results say?

(Privation)

A

Privation is more likely than deprivation to lead to lasting damage.

However, research results are contradictory, with some individuals fully recovering, while others make little if any improvement.

32
Q

Cases of privation are often rare. What is used to research them?

(Privation)

A

Case studies.

33
Q

When did Bowlby conduct his 44 thieves study?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)

A

1944.

34
Q

What did Bowlby’s 44 thieves study aim to test?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)

A

Test the MDH and see whether frequent early separations were associated with a risk of behavioural disorders.

In particular, a disorder deemed “affectionless psychopathy”.

35
Q

How many participants were in Bowlby’s 44 thieves? What were their ages?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

88 children.

5-16 years.

36
Q

What was similar about all the PPs in Bowlby’s 44 thieves?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

All children had been referred to the child guidance clinic where Bowlby worked.

37
Q

How many children in Bowlby’s 44 thieves were referred to the clinic due to stealing?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

44 of the children had been referred to the clinic because of stealing.

These were the ‘thieves’ group.

38
Q

How many (of the 44 thieves) were deemed affectionless psychopaths?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

16.

39
Q

Bowlby identified 16 of these thieves as affectionless psychopaths. What about the others?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

The remaining 44 children in the study had not committed any crimes; they were emotionally maladjusted, but did not display antisocial behaviour.

None of this control group were diagnosed as affectionless psychopaths.

40
Q

What was the PP split in Bowlby’s 44 thieves?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

88 children.

44 referred thieves, 16 being affectionless psychopaths.
44 crimeless children - emotionally maladjusted (no ASB).

41
Q

Of the 44 crimeless control group, how many were affectionless psychopaths?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

None.

42
Q

How did Bowlby get information from the 88 participants?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Procedure)

A

He interviewed the children and their families and was able to build up a record of their early life experiences.

43
Q

What percentage of the 16 affectionless psychopaths had experienced “early and prolonged separations from their mothers”.

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Findings)

A

86%.

44
Q

What percentage of the other thieves (the ones who weren’t classed as affectionless psychopaths) had experienced early and prolonged separations?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Findings)

A

17%.

45
Q

What percentage of the control group (“non-thieves”) had experienced frequent early separation?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Findings)

A

4%.

46
Q

What was concluded from Bowlby’s 44 thieves?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Conclusions)

A

Findings suggest a link between early separations and later social and emotional maladjustment.

47
Q

Does Bowlby’s 44 thieves support the MDH?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Evaluations)

A

Yes, as it suggests that early and prolonged separations from mothers can cause severe antisocial behaviour later in life.

48
Q

In its most severe form, what can maternal deprivation lead to?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Conclusions)

A

Affectionless psychopathy.

49
Q

In its least severe form, what can maternal deprivation lead to?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Conclusions)

A

Antisocial behaviour, e.g. theft.

50
Q

Give 3 evaluative points about Bowlby’s 44 thieves?

(Bowlby’s 44 thieves)
(Evaluations)

A

The evidence is correlational, which means that we can only say that deprivation/ separation and affectionless psychopathy are linked, not that one caused the other.

The data on separation was collected retrospectively and may not be reliable. Parents may not have accurately recalled separations during infancy. They may have over or underestimated the frequency.

How do we know whether these children experienced deprivation or whether they had good substitute emotional care during their separations?

51
Q

Outline Rutter’s research from 1972.

(Challenges Bowlby’s 44 thieves study)

A

Challenges Bowlby’s 44 thieves study.

This is because Rutter stated that Bowlby grouped together a wide variety of experiences under the general heading of maternal deprivation (such as hospitalisation, death of mother and family breakdown) - all of which would have different effects on children.

Rutter argued that an affectionless character was not associated with maternal loss (deprivation) but a lack of love - privation in many of Bowlby’s examples.

This suggests that Bowlby should have correctly operationalised what he was testing for, in order to produce results with a greater accuracy.

Instead, Bowlby could have used a more idiographic approach, rather than a nomothetic approach - as the findings and conclusions being made are very personal to the individuals that were in the study.

52
Q

Outline Spitz’s study from 1945.

(Supports Bowlby’s views on long-term deprivation in the MDH)

A

Supports Bowlby’s views on long-term deprivation in the MDH.

This is because they visited very poor orphanages and other institutions in South America, that looked after children; giving them very little warmth or attention from staff - making the infants apathetic.

Many suffered from anaclitic depression (helplessness and loss of appetite).

This suggests that a lack of emotional care and separation from mother creates severe long-term severe effects

However, the orphanage would most likely create environments of privation - not deprivation.

This challenges the validity of Spitz’s research.

53
Q

Outline Barret’s research from 1997.

(Challenges the PDD model and its lack of acknowledgement for individual differences in short term separation)

A

Challenges the PDD model and its lack of acknowledgement for individual differences in short term separation.

For instance, securely attached children and more mature children cope better with separations.

This suggests that only some children experience distress.

54
Q

Outline Spitz and Wolf’s research from 1946.

(Supports Bowlby’s MDH)

A

Supports Bowlby’s MDH.

This is because they found that the monotropy’s critical period, if exposed to long-term separation would result in permanent physically and mental damage.

This reinforces the severity of infant separation at a young age.

55
Q

Outline Goldfarb’s research from 1947.

(Supports Bowlby’s MDH)

A

Supports Bowlby’s MDH.

This is because they found that infants in an orphanage for more than 3 years were less developed and more aggressive, when tested 12 years after leaving.

This suggests that the breaking of a child’s monotropy (critical period) is detrimental to not only their childhood, but also adulthood lives.

56
Q

Outline Rodgers and Pryor’s study from 1998.

(Supports long-term deprivation early in life and how this leads to poor outcomes for the child)

A

Supports long-term deprivation early in life and how this leads to poor outcomes for the child.

This is because they found that children experiencing two or more divorces have the lowest adjustment rates and the most behavioural problems.

This suggests that Bowlby’s ideas of childhood consequences are carried into adulthood.

It also links to Bowlby’s ideas of an internal working model - as two or more divorces creates a negative ‘blueprint’ for future relationships, perhaps causing the seen behavioural problems.

57
Q

Outline Lewis’ study from 1954.

(Challenges the MDH)

A

Challenges the MDH.

This is because they partially replicated Bowlby’s 44 thieves study on a larger scale, looking at 500 young people; finding that their sample, long-term separation from the mother did not equal criminal behaviour, and thus thieves.

This suggests that there are potentially other factors that may affect the outcome of maternal deprivation at a young age.

Moreover, as Lewis’ study has a large sample size, it helps to decrease the impact of individual differences.

58
Q

Outline ‘situational factors’, as an evaluative weakness of Bowlby’s MDH.

A

Emotional problems shown by children in orphanages and other institutions may have been due to the poor quality of those institutions, rather than the maternal deprivation.

59
Q

Outline ‘interactionalist approach’, as an evaluative weakness of Bowlby’s MDH.

A

It is hard to identify where one factor starts and the other ends

It is unlikely that they work in isolation - and is most likely a combination of both.

Instead: separation, deprivation, and privation should be looked at as a whole - and applied ideographically.

This would create findings and conclusions that are individual and possess greater validity.