Studying for transcription test Flashcards

1
Q

Phonological processes

A

Phonological processes convert a difficult
aspect of phonology into something that is phonologically similar but less difficult
or challenging to produce. The existence or reality of phonological processes is disputed, but the concept has been adopted widely by SLPs simply as a way of describing the systematic error patterns in children’s speech.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Phonological Processes / phonological patterns - according to an SLP?

A

Terms for describing
phonological error patterns in children’s speech. For instance, the term final consonant
deletion is used to describe the pattern of omitting final consonants from words.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What 3 categories are the phonological processes divided into?

A

The processes are
divided into three categories: (1) syllable structure processes, (2) substitution processes, and
(3) assimilatory
processes, in keeping with Grunwell (1987).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Syllable structure processes - what are they?

A

Syllable structure processes describe changes to the syllable structure. Changes can involve

(1) the repetition, deletion, or reduction of an entire syllable,
(2) the deletion or reduction
of consonants in a syllable, or
(3) a change to the order of the sounds within a syllable.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

List 4 common syllable structure processes in typically developing English-speaking children.

A
  1. Weak syllable deletion (post-tonic and pre-tonic) - Omission of an unstressed
    syllable in a disyllabic or polysyllabic word. telephone [tɛləfoʊn] → [tɛfoʊn]) POST-TONIC
  2. Reduplication (complete and partial) - messy /mɛsi/ → [mɛmɛ] COMPLETE // messy /mɛsi/ → [mɛmi]) PARTIAL.
  3. Final consonant deletion - syllable and word final C deleted.
  4. Cluster reduction - (deletion of [usually the marked] C in a cluster. Must still have a C left after reduction, or it is C deletion.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

List 5 less common syllable structure processes in typically developing English-speaking children.

A
  1. Coalescentce: Features associated with two adjacent consonants (typically in a
    cluster) combining into a new consonant. Using the /sp/ cluster as an example,
    coalescence occurs when a child combines the [+ continuant] feature of /s/ with
    the [LABIAL] place feature of /p/, resulting in [f]—which is [LABIAL] and [+ continuant]
    (e.g., spoon /spun/ → [fun]).
    2.Epenthesis - Insertion of a segment (typically schwa) in the middle of a word
    (usually between two consonants that make up a consonant cluster)
  2. Reversal or swapping of the position of two consonants in a word. The
    consonants can be adjacent (e.g., spaghetti /spəɡɛti/ → [psəɡɛti] ) or nonadjacent
    (e.g., animal /ænəməl/ → [æmənəl]).
  3. Migration: The migration or movement of a sound from one position in a word to
    another (e.g., ski /ski/ → [kis]).
  4. Diminutization: The addition of the vowel /i/ or /ɪ/ at the end of a word (e.g., dog
    /dɑɡ/ → [dɑɡi]; bird /bɝd/ → [bɝdi]). This process is associated with “baby talk” and
    may occur during the first 50-word stage of language acquisition.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What is a substitution (systemic) process?

A

Substitution processes describe the changes to consonants or vowels in words. Typically, a
later developing, more marked articulatory feature of a phoneme is replaced with an easier
feature. While it might appear that children substitute or replace one sound for another, the
change usually involves a feature of a phoneme rather than an entire phoneme.
and syllable structure contexts (singleton and consonant
cluster), or they might be constrained to a particular context.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

List 8 common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children:

A
  1. Fronting
    a) Velar fronting
    b) Palatal fronting (deplaltalisation)
  2. Stopping of fricatives.
  3. Stopping of affricates.
  4. Deaffrication.
  5. Gliding of liquids.
  6. Context sensitive voicing (CSV)
    a) Prevocalic voicing
    b) Postvocalic devoicing
  7. Consonant cluster simplification
  8. Fricative Simplification
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Fronting is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of a consonant further back in the mouth with a consonant
articulated further towards the front of the mouth. There are two main types
of fronting: velar and palatal fronting.

a) Velar fronting: Substitution of a velar consonant with an alveolar, such as
/k/ → [t]; /ɡ/ → [d]; /ŋ/ → [n].

b) Palatal fronting (depalatalization): Substitution of a postalveolar consonant
with an alveolar, such as /ʃ/ → [s]; /ʒ/ → [z]; /ʧ/ → [ʦ]; /ʤ/ → [ʣ].

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Stopping of fricatives is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A
Substitution of a fricative consonant with a homorganic (characterized by the same or nearest equivalent
place of articulation)
plosive. 
Examples: /f/ → [p]; 
/v/ → [b]; 
/θ/ → [p] OR [t]; 
/ð/ → [b] or
[d]; 
/s/ → [t]; 
/z/ → [d]; 
/ʃ/ → [t]; 
/ʒ/ → [d
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Stopping of affricates is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of an affricate consonant for a plosive consonant
having a similar place of articulation, such as /ʧ/ → [t] and /ʤ/ → [d] (e.g.,
chew / ʧu/→ [tu]; jam /ʤæm/ → [dæm]). Stopping of affricates is not to be confused
with deaffrication or depalatalization.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Deaffrication is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of an affricate consonant with a fricative such as
/ʧ/→ [ ʃ ] and /ʤ/→ [ʒ] (e.g., chew /ʧu/ → [ ʃu]; jam /ʤæm/ → [ʒæm]). This process is
not to be confused with stopping of affricates or depalatalization.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Gliding of liquids is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of a liquid /l, ɹ / with a glide /w, j/ such as /l/→
[w] or [j], and /ɹ/→ [w] or [j]. Gliding of liquids can occur in singleton and/or consonant cluster contexts (e.g., look /lʊk/→[wʊk]; lamb /læm/ → [jæm]; run /ɹʌn/→
[wʌn]; please /pliz/→[pwiz]; tree /tɹi/→[twi]).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Context sensitive voicing (CSV) is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

The loss of voice/voiceless
contrasts within syllable-initial and syllable-final contexts (Grunwell, 1997):
/p/ → [b], /t/ → [d], /k/ → [ɡ], /f/→ [v], /s/→ [z], /ʃ/→ [ʒ], /ʧ/→ [ʤ]. There are two types:
prevocalic and postvocalic.
(a) Prevocalic voicing: -
voiceless consonants are replaced by the
voiced counterpart in syllable-initial position –> pea /pi/ → [bi];
tea /ti/ → [di]; key /ki/ → [ɡi].
(b) Postvocalic devoicing: Substitution of a voiced consonant with the voiceless
counterpart in syllable-final position –> bib /bɪb/ → [bɪp]; lid
/lɪd/ → [lɪt]; bag /bæɡ/ → [bæk]; love /lʌv/ → [lʌf]; buzz / bʌz / → [bʌs]; badge
/bæʤ/ → [bæʧ].

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Consonant cluster simplification is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of one or more consonants
with an easier consonant. Consonant cluster simplification differs from consonant
cluster reduction because it does not involve the deletion of a consonant. Instead,
it typically involves the substitution of a more marked, later developing consonant
within a cluster, with an earlier developing, less marked consonant in a cluster such
as: /bl/ → [bw]; /dɹ/ → [dw]; /fl/ → [pl] or [pw]; /sk/→ [st]; /kɹ/→ [kw] or [tw]; /ʃɹ/→ [sw]
or [tw]. Typically, the change is the result of another simplification process such as
fronting, gliding of liquids, stopping of fricatives, and/or context sensitive voicing.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Fricative simplification is a common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Substitution of an interdental consonant with a labial consonant,
such as /θ/ → [f] and /ð/ → [v] (e.g., thumb /θʌm/ → [fʌm]; that /ðæt/ → [væt]).

17
Q

List 4 less common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children:

A
  1. Alveolarization (apicalization):
  2. Vocalization:
  3. Labialization:
  4. Stopping of liquids:
18
Q

Alveolarisation (apicalisation) is a less common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Alveolarization (apicalization): Substitution of a labiodental or interdental
consonant with an alveolar consonant such as /f/ → [s], /v/ → [z], /θ/ → [s], and /ð/ →
[z] (e.g., feet /fit/ → [sit]; van /væn/ → [zæn]; thumb /θʌm/ → [sʌm]; that /ðæt/ → [zæt]).

19
Q

Vocalisation is a less common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Vocalization: Substitution of a syllabic (approximant) consonant [w, j, ɹ, l] with a vowel,
–> /l/ → [ʊ]
(e.g., bottle /bɑtl/→[bɑtʊ];
apple /æpl/→[æpʊ]).

20
Q

Labialization is a less common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Labialization: Substitution of a non-labial consonant with a labial (bilabial or
labiodental) consonant, such as /ʧ/ → [f] (e.g., chip /ʧɪp/ → [fɪp]) and
/s/ → [f] (e.g.,
sun /sʌn/ → [fʌn]).

/θ/ → [f]
and /ð/ → [v] is called Fricative Simplification as it is a common substitution pattern
in typically developing English-speaking children’s speech

21
Q

Stopping of liquids is a less common substitution processes in typically developing English-speaking children. Describe it.

A

Stopping of liquids: Substitution of a liquid consonant with a plosive consonant
such as /l/ → [d] and /ɹ/ → [d]
(e.g., run /ɹʌn/→ [dʌn];
lamb /læm/ → [dæm]).

22
Q

List 3 common Assimilation patterns (Consonant Harmony) in typically developing English-speaking children:

A
  1. Velar assimilation (e.g., pack /pæk/ → [kæk])
  2. Labial assimilation (e.g., pig /pɪɡ/ → [pɪp])
  3. Assimilation of voice (equivalent to context sensitive voicing) (Grunwell, 1987)
23
Q

What are Assimilation processes (Consonant Harmony)?

A

Assimilation processes are where one sound becomes more like another sound in the same
word. Three dimensions are used to describe assimilation.
1. Complete assimilation (entire sound assimilated, replacing another) Vs Partial assimilation (feature of sound assimilated ie POA in (e.g., sip /sɪp/ → [fɪp]).
2. Continguous Assimilation (influence
between 2 adjacent sounds) Vs Noncontiguous assimilation (Influence between 2 sounds seperated by (an)other sound)
3. Progressive assimilation (earlier sound affects later sound in word e.g., pig /pɪɡ/ → [pɪp]) Vs Regressive assimilation (later sound in word affects earlier sound e.g., pig /pɪɡ/ → [ɡɪɡ].

24
Q

List 4 less common Assimilation patterns in typically developing English-speaking children:

A
  1. Alveolar assimilation (e.g., pad /pæd/ → [dæd])
  2. Palatal assimilation (e.g., fish /fɪʃ/ → [ ʃɪʃ ])
  3. Nasal assimilation (e.g., lamb /læm/ → [næm])
  4. Liquid assimilation (e.g., yellow /jɛlo/ → [lɛlo]) (this specific example commonly
    occurs in young children’s speech)
25
Q

Working out assimilation patterns in a child’s speech.

A
  1. Determine which sound is influencing another sound in
    the word.
  2. Describe the voice, place, and manner characteristics of the influencing sound to
    identify the type of assimilation (e.g., velar assimilation).
  3. Consider the position of the
    sound in the word to determine whether the influence is regressive (sound later in a word
    influencing an earlier sound) or progressive (sound earlier in a word influencing a later
    sound).
  4. Note whether the influencing and influenced sounds are contiguous (adjacent) or
    noncontiguous (separated by one or more sounds).
  5. Is the assimilation
    is complete (two or more sounds are identical in the child’s production of the word) or
    partial (the influencing sound is not repeated entirely, only a feature of the sound).
26
Q

Initial consonant deletion
(uncommon
and atypical in English, but common and typical in some other languages)

A

Deletion of the initial consonant in syllable-initial
word-initial position (e.g., song [sɒŋ] → [ɒŋ]; feet [fit] → [it]). This process is uncommon
and atypical in English, but common and typical in some other languages

27
Q

Backing
(uncommon and atypical in English-speaking
children, but common and typical in children who are learning to speak some other
languages)

A

Substitution of a consonant further forward in the mouth with a consonant
articulated further back in the mouth. Two types:
a) Backing of velars (velarisation) characterized by /t/ → [k]; /d/ → [ɡ]; /n/ → [ŋ]. This process
is the opposite of velar fronting.
b) Backing of fricatives, characterized by /s/ → [ ʃ]; /z/ → [ʒ]) is the opposite of
palatal fronting (e.g., Sue /su/ → [ ʃu]; zoo /zu/ → [ʒu])

28
Q

Gliding of fricatives

A

Substitution of fricatives with a glide. If this process is present,
sibilants tend to be replaced by [j], and other fricatives replaced by [w] (e.g., see
/si/ → [ji]; feet /fit/ → [wit])

29
Q

Denasalization

A

Substitution of a nasal consonant with a homorganic plosive,
such as /m/ → [b], /n/→ [d] and /ŋ/ → [ɡ]. Some of the words in your own speech are
denasalized (hyponasal) when you have a cold with blocked nose (e.g., me /mi/ →
[bi]; knee /ni/ → [di]; sing /sɪŋ/ → [sɪɡ]).

30
Q

Affrication

A

Substitution of fricatives with affricates (e.g., shoe /ʃu/ → [ʧu]; zip
/zɪp/ → [ʤɪp]).

31
Q

Systematic sound preference

[n/s how this can be differentiated from a phonemic problem where sounds are collapsed ???]

A

Substitution of one speech sound for a range of
other speech sounds, such as using [f] for all fricatives and affricates (e.g., shoe /ʃu/ →
[fu]; zip /zɪp/ → fɪp]) or [d] for all consonants in word-initial position in words (e.g., shoe
/ʃu/ → [du]; zip /zɪp/ → [dɪp]; run /ɹʌn/ → [dʌn]; spoon /spun/ → [dun]; me /mi/ → [di]).

32
Q

Glottal insertion

A

Substitution of a consonant with a glottal stop such as /t/ → [ʔ]
in hat /hæt/ → [hæʔ], happy /hæpi/ → [ʔæʔi] and catching /kæʧɪŋ/ → [kæʔɪŋ]. Glottal
insertion, while not a feature of typically developing speech, can occur in children
with SSD and is considered to be the most common compensatory articulation used
by children with a cleft palate