Chapter 1 Flashcards
(56 cards)
Historical Views of Children and Childhood
Plato (428-347 B.C.) and Aristotle (384-322 B.C.) believed that schools and parents had the responsibility for teaching children the self-control that would make them effective citizens. But both philosophers, particularly Aristotle, also worried that too much self-discipline would stifle children’s initiative and individuality, making them unfit as leaders.
Historical people - education
Plato believed that experience could not be the source of knowledge because human senses are too fallible. He argued, instead, that children are born with innate knowledge of many concrete objects, such as animals and people, as well as with knowledge of abstractions such as courage, love, and goodness. In Plato’s view, children’s sensory experiences simply trigger knowledge they’ve had since birth.
In contrast, Aristotle denied the existence of innate knowledge; instead, knowledge is rooted in perceptual experience. Children acquire knowledge piece by piece, based on the information provided by their senses.
Age of Enlightenment - John Locke
The English philosopher John Locke (1632-1704), asserting that the human infant is a tabula rasa or “blank slate,” claimed that experience molds the infant, child, adolescent, and adult into a unique individual. According to Locke, parents should instruct, reward, and discipline young children, gradually relaxing their authority as children grow.
Locke, like Aristotle 2,000 years before him, believed that children begin these journeys very lightly packed but pick up necessary knowledge along the way, through experience
Jean Jacques Rousseau
Locke’s view was challenged by the French philosopher Jean Jacques Rousseau (1712-1778), who believed that newborns are endowed with an innate sense of justice and morality that unfolds naturally as children grow. In this unfolding, children move through the developmental stages that we recognize today - infancy, childhood, and adolescence. And instead of emphasizing parental discipline, Rousseau argued that parents should be responsive and receptive to their children’s needs.
Rousseau shared Plato’s view that children begin their developmental journeys well prepared with a stockpile of knowledge.
The push toward child development as a science came from two unexpected events in England in the 19th century.
- Industrial Revolution
- in the mid-1700s, England was transformed from rural, relying on agriculture, to an urban-oriented society organized around factories, including textile mills that produced cotton cloth.
- Children moved with their families to cities and worked long hours in factories under horrendous conditions and for little pay.
- Accidents were common and many children were maimed or killed.
- Reformers were appalled at these conditions and worked to enact laws that would limit child labor and put more children in schools. - Charles Darwin’s groundbreaking work on evolution
- He argued that individuals within a species differ; some individuals are better adapted to an environment, making them more likely to survive and to pass along their characteristics to future generations.
- Some scientists of the day noted similarities between Darwin’s description of evo- lutionary change in species and age-related changes in human behavior.
- This prompted many scientists-including Darwin himself-to write what became known as baby biographies, detailed, systematic observations of individual children.
- The observations in the biographies were often subjective and conclusions were sometimes reached on the basis of minimal evidence.
- Nevertheless, the systematic and extensive records in baby biographies paved the way for objective, analytic research.
G. Stanley Hall
Taking the lead in the new science at the dawn of the 20th century was G. Stanley Hall (1844-1924), who generated theories of child development based on evolutionary theory and conducted many studies to determine age trends in children’s beliefs and feelings about a range of topics.
Hall founded the first scientific journal in English where scientists could publish findings from child-development research.
Hall founded a child-study institute at Clark University and was the first president of the American Psychological Association.
Other scientist interests in studying children
Alfred Binet (1857-1911) had begun to devise the first mental tests
In Austria, Sigmund Freud (1856-1939) had suggestion that the experiences of early childhood seemed to account for patterns of behavior in adulthood
in the United States John B. Watson (1878-1958), the founder of behaviorism, had begun to write and lecture on the importance of reward and punishment for child-rearing practices.
the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD)
In 1933, these emerging scientific forces came together in a new interdisciplinary organization, the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD). Among its members were psychologists, physicians, educators, anthropologists, and biologists
SRCD has grown to a membership of more than 5,000 scientists and is now the main professional organization for child-development researchers. It continues to promote multidisciplinary research and to encourage application of research findings to improve children’s lives.
World War II
Progress in the field was halted by World War II, when most child-development scientists in the United States abandoned their research to assist the war effort
But by the 1950s and 1960s, the field was thriving, marking the beginning of the modern era of child-development research.
Applied developmental science
Child-development researchers have learned much during these 50 years.
- Because of this success, a new branch of child-development research has emerged.
Applied developmental science uses developmental research to promote healthy development, particularly for vulnerable children and families
Scientists with this research interest contribute to sound family policy through a number of distinct pathways:
- Some ensure that consideration of policy issues and options is based on factual knowledge derived from child-development research: When government officials need to address problems affecting children, child-development experts can provide useful information about children and their develop- ment
- Others contribute by serving as advocates for children. Working with a child-advocacy group, child-development researchers can alert policymakers to children’s needs and can argue for family policy that addresses those needs.
- Still other child-development experts evaluate the impact of government policies (e.g., the No Child Left Behind Act) on children and families.
- one of the best ways to sway policymakers is to create an actual program that works. When researchers create a program that effectively combats problems affecting children or adolescents (e.g., sudden infant death syndrome or teenage pregnancy), this can become powerful ammunition for influencing policy.
A theory
In child development, a THEORY is an organized set of ideas that is designed to explain and make predictions about development.
A theory leads to hypotheses that we can test in research; in the process, each hypothesis is confirmed or rejected.
When the outcomes of research are as hypothesized, the theory gains support.
When results run counter to the hypothesis, the theory is incorrect and is revised. These revised theories then provide the basis for new hypotheses, which lead to new research, and the cycle continues.
With each step along the way, the theory comes closer to becoming a complete account
Many early theories shared assumptions and ideas about children and development; grouped together they form five major theoretical perspectives in child-development research
biological, psychodynamic, learning, cognitive- developmental, and contextual perspectives
The Biological Perspective
The Biological Perspective - intellectual and personality development, as well as physical and motor development, are rooted in biology
Maturational theory, was proposed by Arnold Gesell (1880-1961).
- child development reflects a specific and pre-arranged scheme or plan within the body.
- In Gesell’s view, development is simply a natural unfolding of a biological plan; experience matters little.
- Like Jean Jacques Rousseau 200 years before him, Gesell encouraged parents to let their children develop naturally.
- behaviors as speech, play, and reasoning would emerge spontaneously according to a predetermined developmental timetable
- Maturational theory was discarded because it had little to say about the impact of the environment on children’s development.
other biological theories give greater weight to experience.
Ethological theory views development from an evolutionary perspective.
- many behaviors are adaptive - they have survival value.
- For example, clinging, grasping, and crying are adaptive for infants because they elicit caregiving from adults.
- Ethological theorists assume that people inherit many of these adaptive behaviors
- Ethologists believe that all animals are biologically programmed so that some kinds of learning occur only at certain ages
- A critical period is the time in development when a specific type of learning can take place; before or after the critical period, the same learning is difficult or even impossible.
- One well-known example of a critical period comes from the work of Konrad Lorenz (1903-1989), a zoologist who noticed that newly hatched chicks follow their mother about. He theorized that chicks are biologically programmed to follow the first moving object that they see. Usually this was the mother, so following her was the first step in imprinting, creating an emotional bond with the mother.
The Psychodynamic Perspective
The psychodynamic perspective is the oldest scientific perspective on child develop- ment, originating in the work of Sigmund Freud (1856-1939) in the late 19th and early 20th centuries.
Freud was a physician who specialized in diseases of the nervous system.
- Many of his patients were adults who suffered from ailments that seemed to have no obvious biological causes.
- As Freud listened to his patients describe their problems and their lives, he became convinced that early experiences establish pat- terns that endure throughout a person’s life.
- Using his patients’ case histories, Freud created the first psychodynamic theory, which holds that development is largely determined by how well people resolve conflicts they face at different ages.
The role of conflict is evident in Freud’s description of the three primary components of personality.
- The id is a reservoir of primitive instincts and drives.
— Present at birth, the id presses for immediate gratification of bodily needs and wants.
- The ego is the practical, rational component of personality.
— The ego begins to emerge during the first year of life, as infants learn that they cannot always have what they want.
— The ego tries to resolve conflicts that occur when the instinctive desires of the id encounter the obstacles of the real world.
— The ego often tries to channel the id’s impulsive demands into socially more acceptable channels.
- The third component of personality, the superego, is the “moral agent” in the child’s personality.
— It emerges during the preschool years as children begin to internalize adult standards of right and wrong.
- Today, scientists recognize many shortcomings that undermine Freud’s theory as a whole
- two of Freud’s insights have had lasting impact on child-development research and theory. First, he noted that early experiences can have enduring ef- fects on children’s development. Second, he suggested that children often experience conflict between what they want to do and what they know they should do.
ERIKSON’S PSYCHOSOCIAL THEORY
- Erik Erikson (1902-1994), Freud’s student, embraced Freud’s idea of conflict, but he emphasized the psychological and social aspects of conflict rather than the biological and physical aspects.
- In Erikson’s psychosocial theory, development consists of a sequence of stages, each defined by a unique crisis or challenge.
- The name of each stage reflects the challenge that individuals face at a particular age.
- Erikson also argued that the earlier stages of psychosocial development provide the foundation for the later stages.
- When children overcome early obstacles easily, they are better able to handle the later ones.
- Psychosocial stage: basic trust vs mistrust. Birth to 1 year. Challenge: to develop a sense that the world is safe and a good place
- Stage: autonomy vs shame and doubt. 1 to 3 years. Challenge: To realize that one is an independent person who can make decisions
- Stage: initiative vs guilt. 3 to 6 years. Challenge: to develop a willingness to try new things and to handle failure
- Stage: industry vs inferiority. 6 years to adolescence. Challenge: to learn basic skills and to work with others
- Stage: Identity versus identity confusion. Adolescence. Challenge: to develop a lasting, integrated sense of self
- Stage: intimacy vs isolation. Young adulthood. Challenge: to commit to another in a loving relationship
- Stage: generativity vs stagnation. Middle adulthood. Challenge: to contribute to younger people, through child rearing, child care, or other productive work
- Stage: integrity vs despair. Late life. Challenge: to view one’s life as satisfactory and worth living
The Learning Perspective
Learning theorists endorse John Locke’s view that the infant’s mind is a blank slate on which experience writes.
John Watson (1878-1958) was the first theorist to apply this approach to child development.
- He argued that learning determines what children will be.
- For Watson, experience was all that mattered in determining the course of development.
- EARLY LEARNING THEORIES.
- SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY
EARLY LEARNING THEORIES
B. F. Skinner studied operant conditioning, in which the consequences of a behavior determine whether a behavior is repeated in the future.
- A reinforcement is a consequence that increases the future likelihood of the behavior that it follows.
— Positive reinforcement consists of giving a reward to increase the likelihood of repeating a previous behavior.
— Negative reinforcement consists of rewarding people by taking away unpleasant things.
- A punishment is a consequence that decreases the future likelihood of the be- havior that it follows.
— Punishment suppresses a behavior by either adding something aversive or by withholding a pleasant event.
However, researchers discovered that children sometimes learn without rein- forcement or punishment.
- Children learn much simply by watching those around them, which is known as imitation or observational learning.
SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY
Children do not always imitate what they see around them.
- Children are more likely to imitate if the person they see is popular, smart, or talented.
- They’re also more likely to imitate when the behavior they see is rewarded than when it is punished.
Albert Bandura (1925-) based his social cognitive theory on this more complex view of reward, punishment, and imitation
- Bandura calls his theory “cognitive” because he believes that children are actively trying to understand what goes on in their world; the theory is “social” because, along with reinforcement and punishment, what other people do is an important source of information about the world
Bandura also argues that experience gives children a sense of self- efficacy, beliefs about their own abilities and talents.
- Self-efficacy beliefs help determine when children will imitate others.
- whether children imitate others depends on who the other person is, whether that person’s behavior is rewarded, and the children’s beliefs about their own talents.
The Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
The cognitive-developmental perspective focuses on how children think and on how their thinking changes as they grow.
Jean Piaget (1896-1980) proposed the best known of these theories.
- He believed that children naturally try to make sense of their world.
- Throughout infancy, childhood, and adolescence, youngsters want to understand the workings of both the physical and the social world.
- Piaget argued that as children try to comprehend their world, they act like sci- entists in creating theories about the physical and social worlds.
- As with real scientific theories, when the predicted events occur, a child’s belief in her theory grows stronger.
- When the predicted events do not occur, the child must revise her theory.
- Piaget also believed that at a few critical points in development, children real- ize their theories have basic flaws. When this happens, they revise their theories radically.
- radical revisions occur three times in development: once at about age 2, a second time at about age 7, and a third time just before adolescence. These radical changes mean children go through four distinct stages in cognitive development. Each stage represents a fundamental change in how children understand and organize their envi- ronment, and each stage is characterized by more sophisticated types of reasoning.
- According to Piaget, children’s thinking becomes more sophisticated as they develop, reflecting the more sophisticated theories that children create.
Stage: sensorimotor. Birth to 2 years. Infant’s knowledge of the world is based on sense and motor skills. By the end of the period, infant uses mental represenations
Stage: Preoperational. 2 to 6 years. Child learns how to use symbols such as words and numbers to represent aspects of the world, but relates to the world only through his or her perspective
Stage: concrete operation. 7 to 11 years. Child understands and applies logical operations to experiences, provided they are focused on the here and now.
Stage: formal operational. Adolescence and beyond. Adolescent or adult thinks abstractly, speculates on hypothetical situations, and reasons deductively about what may be possible
The Contextual Perspective
environment is an important force in development
All these people and institutions fit together to form a person’s culture - the knowledge, attitudes, and behavior associated with a group of people.
- Culture can refer to a particular country or people (e.g., French culture), to a specific point in time (e.g., popular culture of the 1990s), or to groups of individuals who maintain specific, identifiable cultural traditions
- A culture provides the context in which a child develops and thus is a source of many important influences on development throughout childhood and adolescence.
One of the first theorists to emphasize cultural context in children’s development was Lev Vygotsky (1896-1934). A Russian psychologist, Vygotsky focused on ways that adults convey to children the beliefs, customs, and skills of their culture.
Early Development Is Related to Later Development but Not Perfectly
Thus, the continuity-discontinuity issue is really about the “relatedness” of development: Are early aspects of development consistently related to later aspects?
- Do you believe development is a continuous process: According to this view, once a child begins down a particular developmental path, he or she stays on that path throughout life.
- Or do you believe the other view, that development is not continuous?
In reality, neither of these views is accurate. Development is not perfectly pre- dictable.
Development Is Always Jointly Influenced by Heredity and Environment
Virtually all aspects of development are determined by the combined forces of heredity and environment.
What roles do biology (nature) and environment (nurture) play in child development?
Today, we know that virtually no aspects of child development are due exclusively to either heredity or environment. Instead, development is always shaped by both-na- ture and nurture interact.
Children Influence Their Own Development
Children shaped the way in which parents parented.
These two points ofview illustrate the active-passive child issue: Are children simply at the mercy of the environment (passive child) or do children actively influence their own development through their own unique individual characteristics (active child)?
The passive view corresponds to Locke’s description of the child as a blank slate on which experience writes; the active view corresponds to Rousseau’s view of development as a natural unfolding that takes place within the child.
Today, we know that experiences are indeed crucial but not always in the way Locke envisioned.
Often, it’s a child’s interpretation of experiences that shapes his or her development.
Development in Different Domains Is Connected
development in different domains is always intertwined.
Themes
- Continuity: Early development is related to later development but not perfectly.
- Nature and nurture: Development is always jointly influenced by heredity and environment.
- Active children: Children influence their own development.
- Connections: Development in different domains is connected.