Lecture 4: Language, Visuospatial Functions, Normal Brain Ageing Flashcards
(27 cards)
Normal ageing effect on language
- Age effect language functions varies:
Age effect language functions varies:
- Vocabulary maintained with ageing, or even improvement
- Word production, retrieval of word sounds, declines
- Syntactic processing declines
Normal ageing effect on language
- Knowledge word meaning, vocabulary
Knowledge word meaning, vocabulary
- Older adults larger vocabulary than younger adults
- more words represented in the lexicon
- superior performance on NART, providing definitions or using larger variety of words in discourse.
- Vocabulary starts to decline only in 80s or 90s
- not just cohort effect. Longitudinal studies: improved vocabulary until very old age.
Normal ageing effect on language
- 2 subscales Vocabulary (40 items) and Abstract Thinking (20 items)
- from: Verhaeghen (2003)
- WAIS-R Vocabulary
- Vocabulary unaffected by age

Vocabulary unaffected by age
- knowledge
- Knowledge: WAIS Vocabulary and Spot-a-Word test (e.g. flonty – xylophone)
- Longitudinal study: Berlin Aging Study. No deterioration until age 90 (thick line)
- from: Singer et al. (2001)

Normal ageing effect on language
- Language production
Language production
- More difficulties in language production, in particular retrieving the correct word
- Problem not in the semantic system (words not lost)
- Problem in retrieving the correct phonology
(part of) language production system:
- knowledge about words stored in semantic system, transferred into sounds in phonological system to produce words
- Problem in retrieving the correct phonology

Problem in retrieving the correct phonology
- tip of the tongue state (TOT)
- temporarily unable to produce well-known word
- most common with proper names
-
More naturally occurring TOTs in older adults -diary study (Burke et al., 1991)
- however, 97% resolved: information not lost
- Also more TOTs induced in laboratory in older adults
Language production
- Explanation TOT: result insufficient transmission of activation to correct phonological codes
Explanation TOT: result insufficient transmission of activation to correct phonological codes
- Aging would reduce transmission of activation more TOTs.
- Proper names more vulnerable
- lack meaning, few semantic connections
- single link between representation of person and their name
- Baker vs baker
¨Normal ageing effect on language
- Syntax
- understanding and production complex syntax deteriorates with ageing
- linked to impairments in working memory
- Syntax comprehension:
Syntax comprehension
- Older adults perform less well answering questions about sentences with complex syntax
- Older adults less accurate judging plausibility complex sentences (Waters & Caplan, 2005)
- Accuracy associated with WM capacity (e.g. reading span)
- Older adults had poorer WM scores than young adults
- Participants with higher WM capacity were less affected by syntactic complexity.
Normal ageing effect on language
- Syntax production
Syntax production
- In speech older adults produce shorter utterances with simpler syntactic structure than younger adults.
- Vocabulary larger in older adults, but WM poorer
- WM may contribute to age effect in sentence production
Language effects in normal ageing
- in sum:
- Overall, age effect language smaller than memory or EF
- Clear age effect word production (retrieving word from lexicon)
- Syntax comprehension and production declines with age
- Vocabulary improves with age
Visual search tasks:
- Verhaeghen (2017): conjunction search task
- Potter et al (2012): search for target pasta among different types pasta
- Verhaeghen (2017): healthy older adults slower than younger adults in visual search tasks, in particular conjunction search
Visuoconstructional tasks
- Block design (Robins-Wahlin et al., 1993)
- 219 healthy older adults (75-96 yrs)
- Block Design with and without time limit

- Lower scores with higher age
- More accurate without than with time limit
- Difference with and without time limit increased with age

Visuoconstructional tasks
- Clock Setting & Clock Reading (Robins-Wahlin et al., 1993)
- Clock Setting: draw time in empty clock face
- Clock Reading: tell time from position hands
- Clock Drawing Test: empty circle
- Add hours
- Set hands at 11:10

- Clock setting: youngest 2 groups scored better than oldest 2 (p<.05)
- Clock reading: 3 youngest groups scored better than oldest group (p<.05)
- ¨Clock drawing in normal ageing (Von Gunter et al., 2008)
- 51% participants had some difficulty placing numbers correctly in clock
- 33% participants had some difficulty placing hands correctly in clock
- Effect of age – participants over 80, more errors than younger groups, but only when level of education was low
Visuoconstructional tasks
- Clock drawing in normal ageing
- errors CDT represent normal variation
- errors CDT do not need to indicate cognitive impairment
- CDT as screening for dementia: risk of false positives
Normal ageing effect on visuospatial abilities
- Mental rotation
- Review Verhaeghen (2017):
- Review Verhaeghen (2017): mental rotation slowing in older adults
- Old adults slower to mentally rotate shapes and more errors (Dror & Kosslyn, 1994)

Normal ageing effect on visuospatial abilities
- In sum
- Age effects more pronounced on more demanding visuospatial tasks
- e.g. conjunction search, mental rotation (larger angles)
- Healthy older adults make errors on clock tests
Brain changes in normal ageing
- Structural changes
- Volume loss between ages 30 and 90:
- Jernigan et al (2001): cross-sectional, compare structural brain changes 78 healthy adults between 30 and 90 years old
- Volume loss between ages 30 and 90:
- 14% in cerebral cortex
- 35% hippocampus
- 26% cerebral white matter
- Jernigan et al. (2001):
- 1) loss in hippocampus volume accelerated with ageing relative to gray matter losses elsewhere in brain.
- 2) frontal lobes disproportionately affected by cortical volume loss relative to other cerebral lobes
- 3) loss of white matter occurred later than loss of gray matter, but was ultimately greater
Structural changes
- Raz et al. (2005): longitudinal, 5 year follow-up 72 healthy adults (age range at baseline 20 – 77)
- location of volume losses:
- Largest volume loss in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus
- Little volume loss in primary visual cortex

Structural changes
- Fjell et al., (2013). Longitudinal study older adults (55-91):
- Volume changes over 1-year interval
volume reductions:
- Largest volume reductions in frontal and temporal areas
- Least changes around central sulcus and calcarine sulcus
- Within temporal and frontal areas, most decline in:
- medial and lateral parts temporal lobe
- medial and lateral orbitofrontal lobes

Brain changes in normal ageing
- Functional change
- Cerebral blood flow (CBF)
Cerebral blood flow (CBF)
- Reduced resting state cerebral blood flow with ageing
- Highest resting state CBF in visual and motor areas and in superior frontal cortex. Higher CBF in young and middle-aged adults (YA, MA) than in older adults (OA)
- Reduced CBF across whole cortex, average decrease ± 0.45% per year
- specific areas of reduced CBF, include medial temporal lobes, hippocampus, frontal cortex
Functional change
- Cerebral blood flow (CBF)
- during cognitive peformance
Cerebral blood flow (CBF)
- Increase in CBF during performance of cognitive tasks
- Old adults showed greater increase in CBF than younger adults
- or activation in additional areas
- Increase activation would reflect compensation for reduced efficiency in older adults (Grady et al., 1994)
- Increase rCBF in frontal areas in older adults, which is absent in younger adults
Theories of normal brain ageing:
- Account for differences between brain areas in vulnerability to ageing
- Frontal theory of ageing
- Last in first out
- Anterior-to-posterior gradient of vulnerability
Frontal theory of ageing – frontal lobes are particularly vulnerable to effect of ageing
- Parts frontal lobes vulnerable to effect of ageing
Last in, first out – late maturing brain areas are most vulnerable to effect of ageing
- Late maturing medial frontal areas, particularly vulnerable to effect of ageing
Anterior-to-posterior gradient of vulnerability
- Anterior areas (frontal) more vulnerable to effect ageing than posterior areas (e.g. occipital)
Evolutionary account
- Brain areas that developed later in evolution (e.g. prefrontal areas) are most vulnerable to ageing
Frontal/last in, first out/anterior-to-posterior/evolutionary
- Theories in line with early and pronounced decline of frontal areas
- Theories not in line with early and pronounced decline of medial temporal areas
- Maturation completed earlier than prefrontal areas
- Relatively “old” brain areas
Association decline in cognitive function and brain changes with ageing?
- Persson et al. (2012): longitudinal study
- Changes hippocampus associated with changes in memory performance?
- 26 healthy older adults 55 – 79 years,
- Baseline 2002-2003, follow-up 2008-2009
- Tasks: 5 episodic memory tests, including delayed recall and recognition – single sumscore
Activation:
- Hippocampus: persons whose memory performance declined - reduction in activation
- Parahippocampal gyri: persons whose memory performance declined - increase in activation in these areas
Volume
- Hippocampus volume: persons whose memory performance declined over time - smaller hippocampus volume at follow-up

Distinguish brain changes normal ageing from early pathological brain changes
- Overlap in areas affected
- Changes reflection of abnormal ageing in persons in very early stage of dementia (incipient dementia)?
- Possible solution:
- select participants who score same as young participants (“superagers”) or older adults at very low risk of dementia

