Developmental Psychology I: infancy and childhood part 6 Flashcards

(56 cards)

1
Q

Cognitive and Physical Development in Middle Childhood (7–11 years)

Introduction to Growth

A

Key Point:

Children between 7 to 11 years continue to grow at a steady (and relatively slow) rate until the growth spurt in puberty.

📏 Physical Growth:
Children at this age are still growing ~5–8 cm in height and ~2.5–3 kg in weight each year.

During puberty, the growth rate increases to 5–10 cm and 4.5–7 kg per year.

Independence:
At this age, children are more self-sufficient than younger children and not yet troubled by adolescent body changes.

This is a “calm before the storm” stage—mentally and emotionally more stable, physically less volatile than during adolescence.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Attention & Memory Development

A

7 to 11 years of age also sees changes in cognitive abilities.

⚠️ Younger children (2–3 years old):
Have little control over attention.

Example: A toddler watching TV can suddenly get distracted by a cat walking by. Their “attention spotlight” moves on its own, like a motion-detecting security light—easily hijacked.

They are unable to inhibit intrusion of task-irrelevant thoughts.

Imagine trying to focus on a puzzle while someone whispers a joke in your ear. Toddlers can’t “tune it out.”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Role of the Brain – Reticular Formation?

A

The ability to sustain attention gradually improves throughout childhood and early adolescence…

This ties into CNS (Central Nervous System) development.

🧬 The Reticular Formation:
Located in the hindbrain, responsible for sustaining attention, and not fully myelinated until puberty.

Analogy: The reticular formation is like a Wi-Fi router for attention—it helps signal strength and maintain a good connection (attention focus). But before puberty, the “wiring” (myelin) isn’t fully installed yet, so signal drops (distractions) are more frequent.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

🔬 What are LC, LDT, and PPT?

A

These are nuclei—specialized clusters of neurons—that are part of the reticular formation or closely connected to it. Each plays a role in regulating attention, arousal, and cognitive function.

🧠 LC – Locus Coeruleus
Location: Pons (upper brainstem).

Neurotransmitter: Primarily norepinephrine (noradrenaline).

Function: Key role in arousal, attention, and stress responses.

🛎 Analogy:
Think of the locus coeruleus like the alarm bell system of the brain. When something important or surprising happens (like a loud sound), the LC activates quickly and wakes up the brain to pay attention.

🧠 LDT – Laterodorsal Tegmental Nucleus
Location: Midbrain/upper pons.

Neurotransmitter: Acetylcholine.

Function: Involved in alertness, REM sleep, and attention.

💡 Analogy:
The LDT is like a spotlight operator in a theater—it helps control where the brain’s attention beam is pointing, especially when switching focus from one thing to another.

🧠 PPT – Pedunculopontine Tegmental Nucleus
Location: Border of midbrain and pons.

Neurotransmitter: Also acetylcholine, sometimes glutamate.

Function: Helps regulate wakefulness, attention, and motor control.

🎛 Analogy:
Think of the PPT as part of the brain’s control panel for staying awake and alert, especially during tasks that involve movement and attention together (like riding a bike or playing a game).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Memory Task: Miller & Weiss (1981)

A

Ability to resist distraction from task-irrelevant information improves from 7 to 13 years.

Experiment:
Children aged 7, 10, and 13 had to:

Remember locations of toy animals behind screens.

Household items were placed near the screen.

Result:
13-year-olds were most accurate in remembering locations, followed by 10-year-olds, then 7-year-olds.

📌 Why? Older children are better at selective attention—focusing on what’s important and ignoring the irrelevant.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Irrelevant Information Recall

A

13-year-olds were less able to remember the household item compared to 7- and 10-year-olds.

✅ This is a good thing!

13-year-olds were better at ignoring task-irrelevant information.

Analogy: Think of a bouncer at a party—13-year-olds have a better bouncer (brain filter) who only lets in guests (information) that matter.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Meta-Cognition ?

A

They said they should pay attention to the animal and ignore the household item because it was irrelevant.

This demonstrates meta-cognition: the knowledge that someone has regarding their own cognitive processes.

It’s like knowing how you learn best—realizing “I need quiet to study” is meta-cognition.

Even though 7- and 10-year-olds know what to do, they still performed worse than 13-year-olds—knowledge isn’t the same as implementation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Meta-Awareness in Younger Children

A

They are aware of the limits of their mental abilities.

Even preschoolers show meta-cognition!

Meta-Attention:
4-year-olds understand that it is harder to pay attention to two people telling different stories at the same time.

Analogy: Imagine trying to watch two YouTube videos playing simultaneously. A 4-year-old knows one will get missed.

Meta-Memory:
4-year-olds understand that some things are easier to remember than others, and even 3–5-year-olds know remembering many items is more difficult than remembering a few.

Analogy: A child knows it’s easier to remember 3 toys than 30.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Development of Memory Strategies

A

Children of 3–5 years believe that remembering something over a short period is just as easy as over a long period.

🧠 They treat memory like a temporary photo that fades, rather than a long-lasting database.

Around 11 years, children understand the mind holds interpretations (not copies) of reality.

This is key: memory is not a camera—it’s a painter interpreting a scene.

With this increase in understanding comes strategies that promote memory storage and retrieval: mnemonics.

Mnemonic Example:

My Very Easy Method Just Speeds Up Naming Planets
(to remember Mercury → Neptune).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Mnemonic Strategies – Rehearsal

A

Adults regularly use mnemonics. Children develop them slowly.

Two Types:
Rehearsal – Repeating to remember.

Organisation – Grouping similar items.

When 3- and 4-year-olds try to remember objects, they look carefully at them and label them, but do not rehearse.

By 7 to 10 years, children can rehearse, and the more they do so, the more they remember.
__________________________

Rehearsal Style Differences:

5- to 8-year-olds rehearse one word at a time:
e.g., “Lion, lion, lion…”

12-year-olds use active, cumulative rehearsal:
e.g., “Lion, tiger, leopard…” “Lion, tiger, leopard…”

This is like building a LEGO tower. Younger kids add one block repeatedly; older kids build a structured tower by combining blocks.

Younger children may be unable to carry out such sophisticated rehearsal because their limited working memory capacity doesn’t allow clustering.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Organisational Mnemonics

A

Organisation: grouping or classifying stimuli into meaningful clusters that are easier to retain.

🔹 Intuitive Analogy:
Imagine your messy closet. If all clothes, books, and shoes are thrown together, it’s hard to find things (like List 1). But if you group by type—shirts, pants, shoes—it’s easier to remember where things are (like List 2).
This is exactly what the mind does using organisational mnemonics.

Remembering lists 1 and 2 should be equally difficult if you simply use rehearsal.

This means if you just repeat words in your head (rehearsal), both lists are equally hard. But…

By grouping items together into meaningful clusters, list 2 should be easier to remember.

Because:

List 1: Random unrelated items (e.g., Boat, Grass, Pencil).

List 2: Semantically clustered into 3 categories:

Cutlery: Knife, Fork, Spoon, Plate

Clothes: Shirt, Pants, Sock

Vehicles: Car, Boat, Truck

Children appear to use organisation mnemonic techniques from around 9–10 years.

This ability requires abstract categorisation—a skill linked to the Concrete Operational Stage (next part).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

🧱 Concrete Operational Stage (7–11 years)

A

Develop cognitive operations that involve using logic to solve concrete, current problems.

Children at this stage:

Stop thinking purely intuitively (i.e., no longer “just guessing”).

Can mentally manipulate information, but only when it’s about real, concrete things (not abstract ideas yet).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

🔹 Mental Seriation

A

Ability to order things based on quantity, e.g., shortest to tallest.

Preoperational children make:

Incomplete ordering: they skip a few.

Extension errors: they focus on local comparisons (e.g., “this one is taller than the previous”) but miss global order.

Concrete-operational children can make greater-than and less-than comparisons easily.

🔹 Analogy:
Think of ordering books from shortest to tallest.

A preoperational child might say: “this is taller than that” but mess up the full line.

A concrete-operational child can mentally line them all up, like sorting by height in their head.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

🔹 Transitivity

A

Understanding the logical relations between a series of objects.

If A > B and B > C, then A > C.

Preoperational children need to see all objects physically.
Concrete-operational children can rely on logic alone.

Example:
If John > Peter > Sam (in height), a 7-year-old can say John is taller than Sam, even without seeing them side by side.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

🔹 Classification

A

Learn to name and sort objects into categories based on similar characteristics.

Classification is the ability to:

Use logic to make assumptions about objects based on their membership to a category.

Example:
“All mammals are warm-blooded. A horse is a mammal. Therefore, it must be warm-blooded.”

This is deductive reasoning, and children begin to handle it around this stage.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

🔹 Class-Inclusion Task

A

Piaget et al. (2001): 7 daisies, 2 roses → total = 9 flowers

6-year-olds said: “More daisies”, because:

“Daisies are daisies, not flowers.”

They don’t yet understand that categories can be nested (subcategory inside category).

7-year-olds correctly said: more flowers than daisies, showing an understanding of class-inclusion.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

🔹 Conservation & Reversibility

A

Reversibility = ability to mentally reverse an action.

Concrete-operational kids realize that things stay the same even if their form changes (e.g., liquid poured into a different shaped glass).

Also applies to math:
5 + 5 = 10
10 – 5 = 5

Preoperational children can use symbols (e.g., numbers), but concrete-operational children can use logic to operate on them.

“Numbers can be changed and returned to their original state” is an example of reversibility.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

🔤 LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT (Vocabulary and Grammar by Age 6)

A

“By 6 years children have an immense vocabulary (up to 30,000 words) and can form infinitely long sentences and use complex grammar such as the passive voice and subjunctive.”

🔍 What’s Going On?
By age 6, a child can already speak a massive number of words—around 30,000! That’s like knowing almost every word in a small dictionary.

✅ Key Grammar Terms:

Passive voice: “The ball was thrown by John” (the object comes first).

Subjunctive mood: “If I were a bird…” (used for hypotheticals).
________________________________

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

The Pragmatic System: Age 6–11:

A

“Between 6 and 11 years there are great improvements in the pragmatic system.”

🧠 Pragmatic system = how language is used appropriately in social situations (not just knowing grammar or vocabulary, but using language well with others).

“Children become more sensitive to the needs of other people in conversations and show greater awareness of what other people know and do not know.”

💬 Analogy:
Before this age, kids are like radio hosts talking into a microphone—they assume everyone hears what they say. Between 6–11, they start becoming like walkie-talkie users—aware that communication only works when both sides are tuned in and understand each other.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Egocentrism & Theory of Mind (Piaget)

A

“A child < 6… may describe the man as ‘the man’, assuming the other person knows what they know.”

“This kind of mistake is an example of egocentrism.”

📘 Egocentrism (from Piaget): Child assumes everyone sees the world from their perspective.

🧠 Theory of mind: The ability to understand that other people have different knowledge, thoughts, or perspectives.

“They need to introduce ‘a man’… before referring to him as ‘the man’.”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Resolving Misunderstandings

A

“The ability to resolve misunderstandings is also part of the pragmatic system.”

If someone replies with just “What?”, you might repeat your entire sentence.
If they reply “Where’s the nearest what?”, you know to just repeat “metro station”.

🧠 Analogy:
It’s like being in a noisy restaurant. If someone only hears the middle of your sentence, you only repeat the missing part. This takes mental flexibility and is part of social pragmatic competence.

“This ability begins to develop before 3 years of age.”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Emotional Content in Vocalisations

A

“The pragmatic system is not well-developed until school age, and even then, it continues to develop.”

Sauter et al. (2013)… children had to match each vocalisation with a corresponding picture of a facial expression.

5-year-olds were 78% accurate for non-verbal vocalisations and 53% for verbal vocalisations.

10-year-olds were 84% accurate and 72% respectively.

🎭 Emotional understanding: Knowing what emotion is behind a tone of voice—even when the words themselves don’t say it directly.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Humour, Irony, Sarcasm, and Conversation Rules

A

“The pragmatic system… also our ability to understand the relation between language and humour, irony, sarcasm.”

Humour is a social tool. You can’t just know words—you need to “get” what’s funny to someone else.

“Using humour… requires understanding other people’s sense of humour and how words can have double meanings.”

“Certain patterns of speech must be respected.”

🔄 Knock-knock jokes: Highly structured! You break the pattern, you break the joke.

24
Q

Humour & Hearing Loss

A

“Learning to detect humorous content has been proposed to have roots in infant-directed speech.”

👶 Infant-directed speech = exaggerated, musical baby talk (slow, high-pitched, emotional).

“Children with hearing loss, been found to be less sensitive at detecting humour than children with normal hearing suggesting they have had fewer opportunities to detect the subtleties of pragmatics in language.”

👂 If you don’t hear language’s tone shifts, you miss the music of meaning—like watching a movie with no soundtrack.

25
🧠 DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-CONCEPT (Self-Descriptions by Age)
“5 years or under”: “A girl”, “I like cookies” (Just facts: physical features or possessions) “5–9 years”: “I am happy”, “I am brave” (More about personality traits, especially in relationships) “Beyond 10 years”: “I try not to be selfish…” (Private, complex self-knowledge: they start reflecting internally) 🧠 Analogy: It’s like how drawings get more detailed over time. A stick figure becomes a portrait. The self-image deepens.
26
Answering “Who am I?” as Adults
Adults use name, age, gender, nationality, education, etc. Others use abilities or interests (e.g., musician) Some feel pride, others feel shame—this is emotional identity
27
Self-Concept & Social Identity
“The characteristics (physical, mental, emotional, behavioral...) we use to describe ourselves make up our self-concept.” “Social identity is the sense of identity derived from our membership of social groups…” You’re not just an individual—you’re also defined by your belonging.
28
What is Self-Concept and How Does It Develop?
Self-concept and social identity serve as a framework for thinking about us and our social world. Self-concept develops by ‘degrees’, in a gradual and cumulative way (Maccoby, 1980). Our self-concept is under constant review and changes as we progress through cognitive development and accumulate social experiences. 🧠 Intuitive Explanation: Self-concept is like a personal "user profile" in your mind. Imagine a constantly evolving app where you store info about who you are — your likes, skills, personality, and roles. Over time, that profile gets more detailed as you go through life and learn more about yourself. Think of social identity as the "public-facing" part of your profile. It's how you define yourself in relation to others — like being a student, a sister, or part of a sports team. The fact that self-concept develops gradually and cumulatively means it builds like layers of paint on a canvas. Each life event or social interaction adds a new layer — sometimes thin, sometimes bold. And because we constantly review and revise our self-concept as we grow cognitively and experience the world socially, our identity is never static — it’s like software that updates as we process new data.
29
Self-Concept in Early Childhood
🔑 Key Phrases: Asking “Who am I?” reveals stages in self-concept development. Children 3-5 years of age describe themselves using physical/external characteristics, possessions, and activities. Linked to language ability (Eder, 1989, 1990). Even without advanced language, kids show understanding of psychological dimensions like sociability. Suggests self-concept develops before the sophisticated language needed to describe it. 🧠 Analogy: At this stage, kids are like beginner artists trying to draw a self-portrait. Their tools are limited, so they focus on visible things: “I have blue eyes,” “I have a bike,” “I can jump.” These are easy to express with their limited vocabulary. But interestingly, even when language is limited, children still understand themselves in psychological terms — like preferring to play alone. This tells us that self-concept begins in the mind before it can be put into words.
30
Self-Concept in Older Children & Adolescents
🔑 Key Phrases: After 5 years, kids use ‘internal’ characteristics (feelings, attitudes). Rosenberg (1979) studied 8-year-olds and adolescents. Only 36% of 8-year-olds used internal traits (e.g., "shy"). 69% of 14–16-year-olds did. 16-18-year-olds also mention self-control (e.g., “I don’t show my feelings”).
31
Self-Awareness & The Rouge Test
🔑 Key Phrases: Self-awareness is the ability to recognise ourselves as distinct from others. Around 15–18 months, infants show self-awareness in the rouge test. Lewis and Brooks-Gunn (1978, 1979): infants with rouge on their nose looked in a mirror. < 15–18 months: laughed at the mirror image. > 15–18 months: reached for their own nose, showing self-awareness. 🧠 Analogy: Think of self-awareness like realizing you’re the main character in a video game. Before this point, babies see their reflection like it's an NPC (non-player character). But once they recognize, “Hey, that’s ME!” — they unlock the first achievement in self-concept. The rouge test is the “mirror level” — if the baby tries to wipe off the rouge, it shows they know the face in the mirror is their own.
32
Photograph Recognition & Verbal Self-Awareness
🔑 Key Points: Bullock and Lutkenhaus (1990) used photographs to measure verbal self-awareness. Only 18–24 month-olds used their own name, and words like “I” and “me”. 🧠 Analogy: This is like going from understanding your game avatar by sight to being able to talk about it: “That’s me!” Recognizing yourself in a photo and using language to describe it shows a deeper, verbal level of self-awareness.
33
Subjective Self (Lewis, 1990, 1991)?
🔑 Key Ideas: Subjective self = early awareness that: Our actions affect others (agency). Our experience is unique. Our identity is continuous. We have self-reflexivity — we can reflect on ourselves. Measured in the rouge test — shows infants see themselves as unique. 🧠 Analogy: The subjective self is like realizing you’re not just in the story — you’re also writing it. This means understanding you’re a separate agent, your experience is your own, and it’s ongoing (like a movie with a coherent plotline).
34
Early Beginnings in Infancy (Sensorimotor Stage)
🔑 Concepts: In the first few months, infants begin to understand cause and effect in social encounters. Primary circular reactions: self-focused behaviors (e.g., sucking thumb). Secondary circular reactions: external-focused (e.g., hitting toy, crying to get attention). This occurs in Piaget’s sensorimotor stage (1–10 months). Babies gain rudimentary understanding they affect the world around them.
35
The Objective Self
🔑 Concepts: Objective self develops after the subjective self. It means seeing yourself as others see you — recognizing traits like size, gender, ethnicity. It’s the self as an object — a member of a social group. Develops around 2 years of age (Gillibrand et al., 2015).
36
Looking-Glass Self — Social Mirrors
🔑 Highlighted phrases: Children’s developing self-concept is influenced by social factors. We develop a sense of self as we respond to interactions with others and see how other people respond to us. This is the looking-glass self (Cooley, 1904; Mead, 1934). E.g., if someone treats us as trustworthy, we will likely define ourselves as trustworthy. 🧠 Intuitive Explanation: Think of your self-concept as a mirror — but not one you look into alone. Instead, it’s made of reflections from other people’s reactions to you. This is called the looking-glass self: we see ourselves through the eyes of others. If people treat you like you're helpful, you may begin to believe you are helpful. If others ignore you or criticize you often, it can warp your self-view. This happens through repeated social interactions — like play, friendship, praise, or punishment — where children absorb how others evaluate them, and adopt those perceptions.
37
Self-Evaluation and Self-Esteem
🔑 Highlighted phrases: Children evaluate themselves in comparison to others. These self-evaluations (plus others’ opinions) form self-esteem. Younger children have ‘global’ evaluations (e.g., “I am happy”). Older children have specific and differentiated evaluations. 🧠 Analogy: Self-esteem is like a report card you give yourself. Younger kids just give themselves one overall grade — “I like me.” But as they grow, kids begin grading different subjects: Am I good at math? Do others like me? Am I funny? These are specific and differentiated self-evaluations. This is what builds self-esteem, a key part of self-concept — the feeling part of who we are. Example: A child may say, “I’m proud of being a good reader, but I’m not good at sports,” showing differentiated self-evaluation.
38
Ideal Self vs. Real Self
🔑 Highlighted terms: Self-esteem is the result of the discrepancy between: Ideal self (what we want to be, influenced by others' expectations) Real self (what we actually are) When there is little discrepancy, self-esteem tends to be high. High discrepancy suggests the child is “failing to live up to standards”: these standards will not be the same for all children (e.g., some children and some families consider academic performance more important than athletic performance, etc.) From 8–10 years onwards, children show consistent self-esteem.
39
Expectations and Domains of Life
🔑 Highlighted: Self-esteem can only be understood if we consider our own expectations and performance in different domains of life. We don’t value all domains equally. E.g., a child who values sports will feel bad if they fail at it, even if they’re great at math.
40
Attributional Styles (How Kids Explain Success or Failure)
🔑 Key Idea: Children’s self-concept depends on what they believe to be the causes of success and failure. Weiner (1974, 1986) identified four causes: Ability (internal, stable) Effort (internal, unstable) Task difficulty (external, stable) Luck (external, unstable) 🧠 Analogy: When something happens — a good or bad grade — your brain tries to explain why. Imagine you're a detective of your own life, always asking: “What caused this?” You might say: “I failed because I’m not smart” (ability — internal, stable) “I failed because I didn’t study” (effort — internal, unstable) “That test was really hard” (task difficulty — external, stable) “Bad luck” (luck — external, unstable) These beliefs form your attributional style. 🔑 Important Details: Stable causes (ability/task difficulty) → stronger achievement expectancy (confidence that success/failure will continue). Unstable causes (effort/luck) → weaker expectancy (e.g., “I can fix this by trying harder!”)
41
🧠 Optimal Attributional Strategy (Weiner):
It's best to attribute success to ability and failure to effort. Why? Because saying “I succeeded because I’m smart” makes you feel confident. Saying “I failed because I didn’t try” makes you believe you can improve. But saying “I failed because I’m dumb” is harmful — it kills motivation.
42
Age Effects in Attribution
🔑 Key Phrases: Before 7 years: kids are unrealistic optimists. They think they can succeed on almost any task — even after repeated failure. Between 8–12 years, kids distinguish effort and ability more. At 11+ years, schools create ability groups → kids begin linking failure to ability, which is maladaptive.
43
What is the "locus of causality," and how does it affect how children view success or failure?
Locus of causality = Whether the cause of an outcome is seen as internal (from the child) or external (outside the child). 🧠 Internal Causes (child's own traits): Ability → Stable (unchanging) Effort → Unstable (can vary) 🌍 External Causes (not from the child): Task difficulty → Stable Luck → Unstable 🧠 Key Insight (Weiner): We value outcomes more when they are due to internal causes. Why? Because we feel ownership over them — they reflect our effort or our ability, not just external luck or hard tasks.
44
What Are Peer Groups and Why Do They Matter?
Think of a peer group like a team of classmates on the playground. These are kids who are roughly the same age and spend time together regularly. Early in development, peer groups begin to form naturally—like magnets attracting each other on the playground. As children play within peer groups, they start doing more complex things: help children learn social skills like: Emotional regulation (calming down when upset, cheering up a friend) Compromise (agreeing to take turns or share toys) 🔑 Key phrase: help children learn social skills like emotional regulation and compromise By age 5 or 6 (end of the first school year), most children already have a strong ‘belonging’ to a group—they feel “this is my crew.” Then, group dynamics evolve—a bit like a mini-society: You’ll find a leader, followers, and roles forming naturally. Also: Boys and girls go separate ways: Boys: usually form larger groups Girls: tend to form smaller “cliques” 🔑 Key phrases: a strong ‘belonging’ to a group activities in larger groups, cliques
45
What Is Peer Social Status?
Some kids just seem to be naturals at joining groups, others struggle. These relationships are measured as peer social status: the extent to which a child is liked/disliked or accepted/rejected by their peers. Important: Peer group acceptance is not the same as friendship! Friendship = 1-on-1, mutual, reciprocal Peer acceptance = group’s perception of a child 🚨 Why does this matter? Rejection can cause emotional distress, and excluded kids miss out on learning important social skills. 🔑 Key phrases: social status: the extent to which a child is liked/disliked or accepted/rejected by their peers perception of a child rejection can cause emotional distress
46
How Do Psychologists Measure Peer Social Status?
There are three main ways researchers study this: 1. Peer Nomination Children name who they like the most and least. This tells us: Accepted = kids who get the most positive nominations Rejected = most negative nominations Neglected = few nominations either way 🔑 Key phrases: nominate others accepted, rejected, neglected 2. Peer Rating Children rate how much they like or dislike each peer (e.g., on a Likert scale from 1–5). This gives a popularity/acceptance score. 🔑 Key phrase: rate 3. Naturalistic Observation Observers just watch kids in natural settings (like recess) and record: Popular = engaged by many kids Rejected = approach others but get avoided Neglected = stay alone and don’t approach others, and no one approaches them 🔑 Key phrases: Popular (or accepted) Rejected Neglected
47
Why Are Some Kids Accepted and Others Rejected?
✅ Characteristics of Peer Acceptance Kids who are accepted tend to show: Attraction: being physically attractive/larger (like being tall or cute gets attention early on) Academic competence: doing well in school Social skills: warm, friendly, good communicator Cooperative and supportive disposition Also important: Ability to negotiate and compromise Can join different activities and adjust to fit in 🔑 Key phrases: Attraction academic competence, social skills, cooperative and supportive disposition, etc. negotiate and compromise join different activitie _________________________ ❌ Characteristics of Peer Rejection Two types of rejected children: 1. Aggressive, disruptive, uncooperative Think: the class bully or loud troublemaker Lacks social skills Poor at perspective-taking (can’t tell if someone’s teasing or being mean) ___________________________ Timid, withdrawn Think: shy child who avoids joining in May have social anxiety Avoid peers out of fear of rejection, causing a self-fulfilling prophecy 🔑 Key phrases: timid, withdrawn behaviour fear of rejection, self-fulfilling prophecy Also: Many rejected and withdrawn children are aware that they are disliked But: Many aggressive children believe that they are liked by their peers
48
Peer Neglect: Who Gets Ignored and Why?
Neglected ≠ disliked — these kids are just off the radar. Some may lack social skills or prefer solo play on the sidelines Others simply prefer being alone and are not unhappy or lonely 👉 Research shows: Many neglected children are well-adjusted and could join in if they chose to.
49
👪 Family Educational Styles
Think of these styles like parenting recipes. Each recipe mixes two main “ingredients”: 1. Level of affection and communication High = warm, responsive, emotionally supportive 🧸 Example: “I love you, let’s talk about your feelings.” Low = cold, emotionally distant, critical or hostile 🧊 Example: “Stop crying. Toughen up.” 2. Degree of control and demands on children High = strict rules, discipline, clear expectations 🧱 Example: “You must do your homework before TV.” Low = few rules, minimal discipline 🌀 Example: “Whatever, do what you want.” Now, mix and match these two factors to get the 4 Familiar (Family) Styles: Schema: Control & Demands: High High Affection: Democratic Low Affection: Authoritarian _______________________ Control & Demands: Low High Affection: Permissive Low Affection: Indifferent ______________________
50
Democratic (High affection, high control)
Most balanced: warm and structured. Results in: Self-confidence, Good attitude and school performance, Good mental health, Few behavioural problems ✅ Think: "Kind but firm" parent. Like a coach who believes in you but also sets clear rules.
51
Permissive (High affection, low control) Warm but no rules.
Results in: Self-confidence, but Little psychological distress, Behavioural problems and drug abuse 😅 Think: A best-friend parent. “I support you, but you decide everything yourself.”
52
Authoritarian (Low affection, high control)
Strict rules, no warmth. Results in: Obedient and work-oriented, but Sometimes hostile and rebellious, Low self-confidence, Depressive problems 🚫 Think: Drill sergeant. “Do it because I said so.”
53
Indifferent (Low affection, low control)
Cold, distant, uninvolved. Results in: School problems, Psychological adjustment problems, Behaviour and drug abuse problems ❌ Think: Neglectful. “I don’t care what you do.”
54
⚖️ Moral Development (Kohlberg's Theory)
Moral development is about how we learn to tell right from wrong and make ethical choices. Lawrence Kohlberg used stories called moral dilemmas to study this, like the famous Heinz dilemma. Moral dilemma = a situation where you must choose between two or more actions, each of which has both morally good and morally bad aspects. 📘 The Heinz Dilemma: A man named Heinz can’t afford a life-saving drug for his dying wife. The pharmacist refuses to lower the price. Heinz breaks in to steal the drug. 👉 Should Heinz steal the drug? Why or why not?
55
🧗 Kohlberg’s 3 Levels & 6 Stages of Moral Reasoning
🔵 Level 1: Preconventional (4–10 years) (Egocentric: focused on “What happens to me?”) Stage 1A: Heteronomous (Punishment and Obedience) Rule = right. Avoid punishment. “He shouldn’t steal. He’ll get in trouble.” Stage 1B: Instrumental Exchange (Naïve Hedonism) Right = what benefits me (or someone I love) “He should steal the drug because his wife needs it.” ___________________________ 🟡 Level 2: Conventional (10–13 years) (Takes others’ views into account) Stage 2A: Good Boy/Good Girl Morality Right = what pleases others “He should steal it to be a good husband.” Stage 2B: Social System and Law-and-Order Right = obeying laws and maintaining order “He shouldn’t steal because laws must be followed.” _____________________________________ 🔴 Level 3: Postconventional (Adolescence or Adulthood) (Thinks in abstract ethical principles) Stage 3A: Social Contract Right = what’s best for society overall, even if it breaks the law “Stealing is illegal, but justified to save a life.” Stage 3B: Universal Ethical Principles Right = follows deep, personal moral principles (justice, equality) “He should steal. Life is more important than property.” _________________________________ 👀 Two Key Components in Kohlberg’s Theory Social perspective – Egocentric → empathetic → abstract fairness for all. Moral content – What the person believes is right, based on experience and development.
56
🧪 Application: How would kids at each stage reason about the Heinz dilemma?
Level: Preconventional Stage: 1A For Stealing: "He tried to pay; it’s not that bad." Against Stealing: "He’ll get punished, he broke in." _______________________________ Level: Preconventional Stage: 1B For Stealing: "She needs it; he wants her to live." Against Stealing: "It’s selfish; business is business." _______________________________ Level: Conventional Stage: 2A For Stealing: "He’s a good husband." Against Stealing: "If she dies, it’s not his fault." _______________________________ Level: Conventional Stage: 2B For Stealing: "It’s his duty to save her." Against Stealing: "Still illegal, even if it’s love." _______________________________ Level: Postconventional Stage: 3A For Stealing: "Law can’t cover everything; saving a life matters more." Against Stealing: "Ends don’t justify means." _______________________________ Level: Postconventional Stage: 3B For Stealing: "Life and dignity > property." Against Stealing: "Others need the drug too." _______________________________