lecture 13 - map consolidation and human cognitive enhancement Flashcards

(41 cards)

1
Q

CONSTRAINT SATISFACTION

A

Localization discrepancy is mediated by position sense. To
resolve the discrepancy, is position sense distorted

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

TO WHAT DEGREE IS THE RUBBER HAND
INCORPORATED INTO THE BODY SCHEMA?

A

Ehrsson et al., 2007 induced the
rubber hand illusion while in an
MRI scanner
* “Occasionally made brisk
stabbing movements with a
sharp needle toward the rubber
hand”
* Subjects reported feelings of
both ownership of the
rubber hand, and anxiety when
it was threatened.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

….HOW DO MAPS ‘KNOW’ TO STAY
LOCKED INTO A PARTICULAR PATTERN?

A
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

‘GOOD’ PLASTICITY: example

A

REPRESENTATION OF FINGERS IN
THE SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX OF
STRINGED INSTRUMENT MUSICIANS

Elbert et al., Science, 1995
‘GOOD’ PLASTICITY:
REPRESENTATION OF FINGERS IN
THE SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX OF
STRINGED INSTRUMENT MUSICIANS

a representation of those fingers within the primary somatosensory system that seems to correlate with the amount of time people are practising therefore undoubtedly assists in the skill at the instrument

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

‘BAD PLASTICITY’ IN
PATHOLOGIES..
Bleton et al (2011)
dystonia

A

overtrained movement leads to a pathological representation of the hand that is no longer useful

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

‘CONSOLIDATION’ I

A

changes in maps = abstract memory not one that is autobiographical or semantic

looking at implicit memory, procedural, non-declarative = abstract
* Mechanisms that act to STABILISE and ENHANCE
memories over time
* ‘Memories’ do not have to be DECLARATIVE: NON-
DECLARATIVE/PROCEDURAL/IMPLICIT memory.
* The changes to cortical map structure are therefore a kind
of ‘MEMORY’

when talking about this in terms of autobiographical stuff there will be some sort of sensory info coming in that will be put into some sort of short term buffer through some process of rehearsal, you can keep it in the short term buffer but also go through some process of consolidation and pass it into long term memory and you can also get it back from there

short term buffer = short term map representation the rehearsal may be some continual pattern of stimulation coming in for it and then some process turns it into a longer term map structure that is not as easy to change passed on short exposures to sensory info

diagram in notes

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

‘CONSOLIDATION’ II

A
  • ‘Consolidation’ requires a SIGNAL – to ensure changes in
    the map that are of of VALUE /BEHAVIOURAL
    SIGNIFICANCE/ RELEVANCE survive.
  • One system whose role in this process has been studied is
    the BASAL FOREBRAIN -> CORTICAL PROJECTION
    system. - a very diffuse projection system , subcortical coming from a number of distinct nuclei

basal forebrain made up of a small number of neurons deep within the brain but can signal to vast amounts of the brain, also can’t only effect receptors in its area aswell due to neurotransmitters

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

‘CONSOLIDATION’
III

A

Within the basal forebrain is the
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert (NBM).
* Neurons within the NBM use
acetylcholine (Ach) as their
neurotransmitter.
* The projection from the NBM is the
MAJOR SOURCE OF
CHOLINERGIC (neurons using
Ach) INNERVATION to the
CORTEX.
* ACh is both a neurotransmitter AND a
NEUROMODULATOR – a substance
that can regulate the activity/firing of
larger groups of neurons.
* COULD THIS BE THE SIGNAL?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

LINKING THE NBM AND ACH TO
MAP PLASTICITY

A
  • Lesion Studies = early studies
    talking about lesioning certain parts of the brain and seeing what happens after they are taken out
  • Juliano, Ma, Eslin (1991)
  • REMOVE NBM, cortex is ‘STARVED’ of
    Cholinergic input.
  • PREVENTS MAP EXPANSION AFTER
    DIGIT AMPUTATION

NBM INTACT
NBM
REMOVED
NO ACH
PROJECTION
TO MAP

everything in the Map stays in their zones

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

..BACK TO ’THE MERZ

A
  • AUDITORY cortex plasticity this time (Kilgard and Merzenich,
    1998), in primary auditory cortex (A1).
  • SO not a map of the body (SOMATOTOPY) but a map of sound
    FREQUENCY (TONOTOPY

auditory space = a map of frequency this is tonotopy

can do similar experiments and use rodents as have similar cortex for auditory

the experiment was mapping A1 to begin with you give them various tones and you record it from A1 using a electrode so we get an order tonopy from low to High frequencies that span the map
each colour in image represents the area within the that best responds to that given frequency

rats can hear a lot higher than we can hear
we can hear from about 20 to 20kHz

once you have a stable map you try and mess with it

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN Kilgard and Merzenich, 1998

A

1 - Naïve’ Rat A1 Organisation
2 - rat brain stimulation and a sound
3 - Tone+ NBM Stimulation Rat A1 Organisation

wanted to pair stimulation in the NBM with a sound simulatneously and then looked at what happened to the rats organisation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

NBM STIMULATION+ TONE RESULTS
IN MAP REORGANISATION

A

graphs in notes

in the normal rat A1 there is a nice progression between the different frequencies

after they have paired a 9kHz tone with the NBM cholinergic signal (trying to work out does it signal something to do with value or behavioural relevance - it appears to as there is a massive reorganisation around 9kHz- there is a massive over representation of 9kHz

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

CHOLINERGIC INPUT MODULATES
THE WAY THAT CORTICAL
NEURONS RESPOND TO SENSORY
STIMULI

A

image in notes

if you only have a little bit of basal forebrain input and your mapping that region you get a little bit of activity not a lot, once you have active stuff in there you have a lot of activation, particularly in spatial areas related to the tone and that seems to somehow drive this map reorganisation

not ethological

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

NBM+MAP PLASTICITY IN
NATURALISTIC LEARNING

A
  • Kilgard and Merzenich linked map plasticity, NBM activity
    and plasticity, but there was no real LEARNING – this was
    a PASSIVE paradigm (the rats performed no TASK)
  • We would like to link MAP CHANGES, FOREBRAIN(NBM)
    ACTIVITY, and CHANGES IN PERFORMANCE on an
    actual task. - if not we may have a epi phenomena = a change in the map thats occurred but we have no idea what it means
  • Connor and colleagues (2003) lesioned ONLY cholinergic
    forebrain(NBM) neurons projecting to the cortex using a
    targeted immunotoxin that ‘recognised’ these neurons.
  • MORE SPECIFIC than either the Juliano et al lesions (takes
    the whole NBM) or the Kilgard and Merzenich stimulations
    (primarily activating cholinergic neurons, but NO REAL
    WAY OF TESTING THIS….
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

NBM+MAP PLASTICITY IN
NATURALISTIC LEARNING

A
  • Behaviourally, NBM lesion group had
    LOWER ACCURACY and TOOK
    LONGER TO ACQUIRE the reaching
    task skill.
  • Used mapping to look at MOTOR
    CORTEX in both groups of rats after
    training
  • The cortical area that elicited forepaw
    movement was increased by 30% in sham
    lesioned rats compared to untrained rats. - training increases the motor map
  • In contrast, the forepaw area of NBM
    lesioned rats actually decreased by 22%. - the area has got smaller

One of the first studies to directly link impairments in learning with disruption of
cortical plasticity after a forebrain lesion
due to Ach in this case but other signals and neurotransmitters must also do this job

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

NBM+MAP PLASTICITY IN
NATURALISTIC LEARNING - motor topic map

A

which is the representation of the muscles and movements within a space
* Connor and colleagues (2003) trained rats to retrieve sugar
pellets through a small slit using a single forepaw.
* One group NBM lesioned, one group ‘sham’ lesioned
(similar surgical experience but no immunotoxin) = pretty good control as have the handling, the stress, the injection and recovery just without the immunotoxin

involves motor learning

reach training
training induced motor map expansion + high accuracy reaching

reach training with NBM lesion
lack of motor map expansion + low accuracy reaching
as no cholinergic input

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

ACH IS CERTAINLY
IMPORTANT…

A

Multiple studies link the NBM and ACh to changes (or not
after lesioning) to cortical map structure.
* Sensory input from the environment may signal HOW to
learn (synchronous input demonstrates things are
‘happening together’ outside, and so maybe should be
linked in how they’re stored ‘inside’ (the map structure).
* But ACh may be the signal that determines WHAT to learn
* The release provides a ‘window’ where stimuli that
arrive during that time are deemed ’more important…’ - and may be able to change the Map in that period

16
Q

…BUT IT’S NOT THE ONLY GAME IN TOWN

A

….ARE THERE QUICKER WAYS TO IMPROVE
WITHOUT RELYING ON TRAINING AND
CONSOLIDATION? cognitive enhancers

17
Q

THE DAWN OF COGNITIVE ENHANCEMENT

A

examples in notes
modafinil + Ritalin - were designed for particular pathologies but are now used extensively for off prescription things - both seem to have effects in neurotypicals

Ritalin increase concentration but doesn’t disrupt sleep patterns

modafinil removes the need for sleep to improve clarity

need to be looked at more in neurotypcials as are drugs of abuse

the effects of them are rather limited, have to go through blood Brain barrier, they have side effects and your using them for purposes that they are not exactly designed for, so they are going to be very variable in neurotypicals

for neurotypicals a better way if we want to get in and influence the brain given that the brain is electrochemical is to dump the chemical and just go straight to the electric

18
Q

use direct electrical Brain stimulation to noninvasively modulate brain function

A

one day in the future woman prescribe electrical stimulation in a similar way to drugs

19
Q

ZAPPING THE BRAIN:
A HISTORY

A

Scribonius Largus
– 43-48 AD (torpedo fish)
– On the scalp for headache

  • Galen, 131-410 AD
  • Ibn-Sidah, 11th century
    – Electric catfish
  • Studied by Walsh, 1773
    – Begins electrophysiology
    – Followed up by
  • Galvani
  • Volta
    G + V came up with modern theories of electromagnetism
20
Q

…AND THE NEXT GENERATION
Giovanni Aldini, Galvani’s Nephew, 19th Century

A

applied galvanism/shock to 27yr old farmers head who had been suffering from major depression/ melanchonic madness in 1801 - appeared to cure him at least in the short term

he also put voltage through dead bodies and made the muscles jerk

first historical example you can non-invasively trace a neuropsychiatric pathology through non-invasive scalp stimulation with direct current

but the idea of how you do it had to wait for the idea of functional localisation in the brain

21
Q

FRITSCH AND HITZIG, 1870

A

First to discover that electrical
stimulation of the cortex can
produce movements.
* Exposed surface of dog’s brain
to electrical stimulation, noted
where this produced
movement.
* Don’t try this at home? They
did…
Δ, twitching of neck muscles; +, abduction of foreleg; †, flexion of foreleg; #,
movement of foreleg; diamond,facial twitching

also found if you reverse the polarity of the current you get opposite movements

22
Q

SUMMARY OF EARLY
STIMULATION WORK

A
  • Direct current can be
    applied to the body and
    brain. Aided in
    development of doctrine
    of functional localisation.
  • After Fritsch and Hitzig, it
    was appreciated that
    application of a positive
    d-c stimulus to the surface
    of the cortex had a
    stimulating effect, while a
    negative current inhibited
    it.
  • Aldini’s work was first to
    suggest that direct
    current applied to the
    scalp could modulate
    human brain function – in
    this case, depression.
  • Largely forgotten after
    the rise of ECT –
    electroconvulsive
    therapy
22
BASIC FEATURES OF TRANSCRANIAL ELECTRICAL STIMULATION (TES)
* Equipment: surface electrodes, constant current stimulator (battery source). * Stimulator: 0-4mA, usually 1- 2mA used . * Electrodes: Size varies (5- 50cm2), usually soaked in conductor (saline) * ‘tES’ - the umbrella term. ‘tDCS’ – one particular stimulation paradigm (using direct current) 2mA about 1/500 amount used by a 100W light bulb diagram in notes quite a basic machine with some electrodes that are stuck on the head what has changed is our understanding of how the brain works rather than how direct current influences it
23
META-ANALYSES (HORVATH ET AL., 2014/15)
-Of the 59 analyses undertaken, transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) was not found to generate a significant effect on any. * ‘Taken together, the evidence does not support the assertion that a single- session of tDCS has a reliable effect on cognitive tasks in healthy adult populations.’ * Main finding to take away from their papers – changes in MEP (motor evoked potentials, a way to evaluate motor cortex) amplitude after motor cortex stimulation seem to be the most reliable finding in the tES literature - still useful for recovery after stroke
24
META-ANALYSES (HORVATH ET AL., 2015/16)
* BUT….this was a slightly unusual meta- analysis. * The authors decided to only include results that had at least one published independent replication attempt. * One hundred and seventy-six articles were excluded as a result. * A re-analysis including these papers would be extremely useful…
25
SO WHY ARE TES’ EFFECTS SO VARIABLE?
Differences in cortical anatomy between participants Differences between electrode positioning between participants State- dependent effects of cortical activity between participants tES is SIMPLE; yet the BRAIN is complex…
26
"Experimental Social Psychology" by Kurt Lewin, published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS
Summary: 1. Scientific Approach to Social Phenomena: Lewin argues for the application of scientific methods—especially experimentation—to study complex social problems. He emphasizes the need to move beyond descriptive or purely theoretical approaches to more empirical, data-driven analyses. 2. Field Theory: He introduces and elaborates on field theory, which views behavior (B) as a function of the person (P) and their environment (E), formally expressed as: B = f(P, E) This approach highlights the importance of understanding individuals in the context of their environment or “life space.” 3. Importance of Group Dynamics: Lewin underscores the relevance of group dynamics, proposing that social groups are systems whose behavior cannot be reduced to the behavior of individuals alone. Group behavior must be studied in terms of the whole group's structure and processes. 4. Action Research: He advocates for action research, a method of simultaneously studying and trying to solve real-world social problems. Lewin describes how this approach is useful in areas like leadership training, reducing prejudice, and organizational change. 5. Experimental Design and Ethics: The article also touches on challenges in experimental design for social studies, particularly ethical concerns, complexity in controlling variables, and difficulties in replicating real-world conditions in the lab. 6. Practical Applications: Lewin illustrates how experimental social psychology can be applied to solve pressing societal issues—such as promoting democratic leadership styles, improving productivity, and addressing intergroup conflict. Impact: Lewin’s article is foundational, advocating a rigorous, scientific, and practical approach to understanding social behavior. His concepts—especially field theory, group dynamics, and action research—remain influential in psychology, education, organizational development, and beyond
27
* Kilgard and Merzenich Cortical Map Reorganization Enabled by Nucleus Basalis Activity
Little is known about the mechanisms that allow the cortex to selectively improve the neural representations of behaviorally important stimuli while ignoring irrelevant stimuli. Diffuse neuromodulatory systems may facilitate cortical plasticity by acting as teachers to mark important stimuli. This study demonstrates that episodic electrical stimulation of the nucleus basalis, paired with an auditory stimulus, results in a massive progressive reorganization of the primary auditory cortex in the adult rat. Receptive field sizes can be narrowed, broadened, or left unaltered depending on specific parameters of the acoustic stimulus paired with nucleus basalis activation. This differential plasticity parallels the receptive field remodeling that results from different types of behavioral training. This result suggests that input characteristics may be able to drive appropriate alterations of receptive fields independently of explicit knowledge of the task. These findings also suggest that the basal forebrain plays an active instructional role in representational plasticity
28
"Neural Correlates of a Perceptual Decision in the Parietal Cortex (LIP) of the Rhesus Monkey" by Shadlen and Newsome, published in Neuron (2001).
Summary Objective: The study investigates how neurons in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) of the rhesus monkey encode information related to perceptual decision-making, specifically when interpreting noisy motion stimuli. Background: Perceptual decisions are those based on sensory evidence (e.g., deciding the direction of motion). The LIP area is known to be involved in eye movements and attention. The authors explore whether LIP neurons also reflect the evolving decision process, not just the sensory input or motor output. Methods: Rhesus monkeys performed a motion direction discrimination task using a random-dot kinematogram. Monkeys viewed a screen with moving dots and were trained to indicate the direction of motion by making a saccadic eye movement to a target. Researchers recorded from single neurons in the LIP during task performance. Key Findings: LIP neurons show gradually increasing activity during the decision period, particularly when the motion stimulus favors the neuron's preferred saccadic direction. This activity accumulates evidence over time, consistent with a drift-diffusion model of decision-making. The rate of activity increase correlates with the strength of the motion signal (motion coherence) and the decision time. The LIP activity appears to represent an internal decision variable, not just sensory input or planned movement. Conclusions: Neurons in the LIP integrate sensory evidence over time to contribute to perceptual decision-making. This supports a model in which decision-related neural activity builds up until a threshold is reached, triggering a behavioral response (e.g., an eye movement).
29
The article "Neural correlates of perceptual decision in parietal cortex" by Shadlen and Newsome investigates how neurons in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) of the monkey brain contribute to perceptual decision-making
The authors studied monkeys performing a direction discrimination task with moving dot patterns and found that LIP neurons reflect both the sensory evidence and the evolving decision. Key findings include: LIP neuron activity gradually increases or decreases depending on the strength and direction of motion, suggesting these neurons accumulate evidence over time—a process akin to evidence integration in decision-making models. The neuronal firing rate correlates with both the monkey's choice and the confidence in that choice. The results support the idea that perceptual decisions emerge from the temporal integration of noisy sensory evidence, and LIP serves as a critical site for this computation. Overall, this study links neurophysiological data with computational models of decision-making, providing insights into how abstract cognitive processes are implemented in the brain
30
Evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) generates little-to-no reliable neurophysiologic effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation in healthy human subjects: A systematic review horvath et al 2015
Abstract Background: Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a form of neuromodulation that is increasingly being utilized to examine and modify a number of cognitive and behavioral measures. The theoretical mechanisms by which tDCS generates these changes are predicated upon a rather large neurophysiological literature. However, a robust systematic review of this neurophysiological data has not yet been undertaken. Methods: tDCS data in healthy adults (18-50) from every neurophysiological outcome measure reported by at least two different research groups in the literature was collected. When possible, data was pooled and quantitatively analyzed to assess significance. When pooling was not possible, data was qualitatively compared to assess reliability. Results: Of the 30 neurophysiological outcome measures reported by at least two different research groups, tDCS was found to have a reliable effect on only one: MEP amplitude. Interestingly, the magnitude of this effect has been significantly decreasing over the last 14 years. Conclusion: Our systematic review does not support the idea that tDCS has a reliable neurophysiological effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation - though important limitations of this review (and conclusion) are discussed. This work raises questions concerning the mechanistic foundations and general efficacy of this device - the implications of which extend to the steadily increasing tDCS psychological literature. Keywords: Electroencephalography (EEG); Event related potential (ERP); Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); Neurophysiology; Systematic review; Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS); Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
31
Early Views on Brain and Behavior
Ancient beliefs: Heart was seen as the center of thought/emotion (e.g., Egyptians, Aristotle). Hippocrates & Galen: Argued the brain is responsible for thought and senses. Galen: Dissected animal brains; showed sensory nerves connect to brain, not heart.
32
Descartes & Reflexes
Proposed mind-body dualism: mind is separate but interacts with body via pineal gland. Believed in reflexes: automatic responses to stimuli (e.g., pulling away from heat). Used hydraulic models (inspired by garden statues) to explain movement. Wrong about fluids in nerves, but introduced testable mechanistic models.
33
Galvani's Discovery
Showed electricity, not fluid, activates nerves/muscles. Sparked the study of electrophysiology and neural communication.
34
Johannes Müller
Introduced doctrine of specific nerve energies: same electrical signal interpreted differently depending on the brain area (e.g., optic vs. auditory nerves). Promoted experimentation over passive observation.
35
Experimental Ablation
Pierre Flourens: Removed animal brain parts to study function loss (e.g., movement, reflexes). Paul Broca: Linked speech loss to damage in left frontal cortex (now "Broca’s area"). Showed localization of brain function, though many processes (like speech) involve multiple regions.
36
Fritsch & Hitzig
Stimulated dog’s motor cortex; observed muscle movements on the opposite body side. Identified primary motor cortex and its role in voluntary movement.
37
Helmholtz
Measured nerve conduction speed (~90 ft/sec), slower than expected. Proved nerve signals are biological, not purely electrical like wires. Contributed to theories of vision, hearing, and energy conservation.
38
Modern Advances
Inventions include: Amplifiers for electrical signals Neurochemical techniques (analyzing brain chemicals) Histological tools (viewing brain cells and structures)