Lawyer Skills Flashcards

(82 cards)

1
Q

Role confusion

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
What I have found, however, is that, for many of
my clients, even the teenagers among them, the message does not sink in. They
continue to assume that I will take whatever action I think is right, and that
I stand united with the public child welfare agency in controlling their fate.
For many of my clients, despite my frequent protestations to the contrary, I am
a part of the all-powerful “you all” that gives and takes away placements, vistis,
and services as we see fit.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Role confusion effects

A

. A
client may run away from a foster care placement without ever having called me
to explain the problems he is having or to consult about his options. A client
may withhold a critical piece of information under the false impression that I
will support his position only if I agree with it. A child may simply not
commit the time, energy, and heartache required from an effective client-lawyer
consultation process.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What will consciencious lawyer first do?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Conscientious lawyers for children in the
dependency and custody context will try to allow children’s confusion by
explaining their role.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

How does lawyer bridge presumed power gap? How will kids react to this?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
To bridge this gap, the conscientious lawyer
will describe what they intend to do for the child.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
A lawyer assuming the traditional role will
explaint aht it is their job to make sure the judge knows what the child thinks
abou t the issue before the court, and that she will press for what the child
wants, whether or not she agrees with the child’s position. To a younger child,
who has little understanding of the court system, particularly as it applies to
his situation, the described lawyer-judge conversation may make no sense. And
although an older child is in a better position to comprehend the lawyer’s
explanation, his greater sophistication will also inspire a healthy skepticism:
he will be likely to disbeileive an adult professional’s representation that
she will cede control to a child.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What has happened to children secrets in the past?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Children who have told secrets to therapists,
doctors, or teachers about parental abuse and neglect have often already seen
those secrets divulged, first to protective service agency and then to the
court and the very people they “betrayed.”</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Impact of lawyer knowing secret information before being told it by kid?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Finally, their very introduction to their
lawyers, who come armed with considerable “secret” information, alerts children
to the lawyers’ willingness and ability to access and divulge information
without their knowledge or consent.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What happens if child doesn’t understand traditional attorney role?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
If the child doesn’t understand the traditional
attorney’s role, the attorney cannot effectively represent their client. Unless
the child client undertstands that his lawyer will zealously advocate his
positions, he will have no incentive to invest the time in client-lawyer consultations
necessary for good, informed decision making, let alone the incentive to turn
to his lawyer for advice, including advice involving confidential matters.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What determines how much info a child gives their lawyer?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
The role debate concerning the representation of
children often seems to rest on an assumption that what a child wants and what
he knows is a prepackaged set of information ready to be delivered to whomever
asks (so long as the asker uses age-appropriate language in a child-friendly
setting). In fact, however, the shape and size of the package delivered, not to
mention how frequently and aggressively the child will make deliveries, will be
heavily influenced by the child’s perception of who is asking, and what that
person intends to do with the goods.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
As with adults, a child’s willingness to develop
a relationship and share information within that relationship will depend
largely on his understanding of the relationship and how the information will
be used.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
If a child understands nothing about his lawyer
to distinguish her from the grocer, he will be, most appropriatelay and
understandably, reticent. He will want to put a good face on a bad situation,
and get out of the conversastion as quickly as possible.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
If, as is common, the child confuses his lawyer
with the agency responsible for separating him from his family or sending his
mother to jail, he will likely choose to emphasize information that will change
those outcomes (such as information about all the good things his mother does),
or he will focus his discussion on important matters he understands to be
within the lawyer’s expertise (when are you all going to let my mother out of
jail).</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Why are children typically keeping secrets in childwelfare cases?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Unlike adults, who tend to be concerned with
revealing information that might implicate themselves in wrongdoing, children
are more often concerned with keeping secrets to protect others (such as
parents). In addition to fearing the legal consequences of the potential
revelation (such as the arrest of their parents, their placement in foster care
or their separation from siblings), children fear the emotional consequences as
well. They fear that they may disappoint, anger, or lose the love of the most
important people in their lives.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What is necessary for child to share secrets with lawyer? Impact?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
There willingness to speak candidly to their
lawyers about matters in questin will therefoe be affected by their sense of
whether secrets will be kept. When a child is convinced that his secrets are
safe with his lawyer, he is likely to share information more candidly, and the
greater candor, in turn, will enhance his lawyer’s ability to assess the mertis
of his case, provide good legal advice, and advocate effectively on his behalf.
On the other hand, when the child client believes that his lawyer is prepared
to share his secrets at will, the child may take care to reveal only
information he is willing to share with the world at large.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Hardest concept for children to understand about lawyer?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
The hardest concept for the child to understand
will not be that his lawyer wants to hear his views, but that these views
amount to a direction of the lawyer’s actions. Unlless the lawery’s
communications also explain why and how the views expressed by the child will
affect what the laywer does, the child cannot be said to be “participating,”
let alone intelligently.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What happens if child doesn’t understand they are the one giving the direction?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Moreover, unless the child understands that he
controls decision making, his lawyer can easily manipulate their discussions to
glean whatever “direction” she wants from the client.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
A child’s convewrsation with his lawyer about
how he likes a foster home, or school, or his mother’s boyfriend, can easily
turn into a lawyer led discussion, where the child must directly contradict his
lawyer in order to take over “direction” of the representation. The laywer will
leave the conversation convinced that the child’s position is in accord with
her own, when, in fact, the child was unaware of taking any position at all. All
he thought he was doing was politely hearing the lawyer out.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Why don’t children expect their secrets to be kept? Impact?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Children generally have no reason to expect that
adults will keep their secrets. Indeed, many children have become involved in the
court system, precisely because they share secret information about their
parents’ misdeeds with doctors, teachers, or social workers who passed on that
information to courts, other social woerks, and their parents thmesleves.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Given this life experience, children will be
slow to understand (and, more importantly, believe) this dimension of their
lawyers’ role. Without that understanding, they will be disinclined to trust
their lawyers enough to share information as freely as is contemplated in the
normal client-laywer relationship; that is, as freely as necessary to ensure
high quality representation and effective client-lawyer consultation.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Impact of knowing secrets are kept on child’s understanding of the lawyer’s role?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
A child’s understanding of his lawyer’s duty to
keep his secrets is key to his true understanding of the lawter’s entire role.
By peding to maintain a child client’s secrets, a lawyer sends the child the
most powerful, comprehsible message about child control.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

One
of the central reasons children have such difficulty understanding and
accepting the traditional attorney’s role is that

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
despite what they are told, their experience
tells them to disbelieve and to distrust. Informing children that you, their
adult attorney, are required as part of tyour job to keep all their secrets
will strike them as extraordinary. IN my experience, this information causes
children to sit up and take notice in a way that the time-worn “I’m here to
help you,” or “to tell the judge what you want” never will. This is, in part,
because a pledge of secrecy is unusually comprehensible to a child.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Unlike general discussions of client control,
the specifics of the confidentiality obligation can easily be put into lanauge
a child can understand.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Children understand the concept of keeping
secrets very well, and the concept of a lawyer “getting in trouble” if she
tells secrets, for these concepts are part of the ordinary social world of a
child.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Secret pledge with child

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
In pledging to keep secrets, the laywer promises
to follow the rules that children ordinarily can only impose on one another. In
making the pledge, she signals her willingness to cross the power line between
child and adult.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Far more important to a child’s appreciation of
his lawyer’s role than the pledge of secrecy is the honoring of that plefge. When
a lawyer demonstrates that she will keep her client’s secrets, even the darkest
family secrets, she will earn her client’s trust and convince her client, if
anything can, that he is, in fact, in control.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Impact of parents access to info

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
. In most cases, parents, even those whose
children have been removed from their care, have a right to access a child’s
mental health records, school reports,a dn child welfare records.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Moreover, no confidentiality rule confines
disclosure by the parents, so they are free to share this information, as well
as any secrets told them by their children, with anyone they please.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

What will the lawyer look like to the child until the child understands they give direction?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Until the child understands that his lawyer
comes, and must come, to him for direction concerning her actions in his case,
the lawyer will remain, the child client’s eyes, just another adult controlling
his life, in the name of helping him out.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

The
lawyer must explain exactly who he represents, when dealing with constituents… 

A

who
might otherwise be misled. No other constituent is more vulnerable to being
misled than a child whose lawyer talks about representing him, when in fact she
owes him no duty of loyalty.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

kids understganding of professional roles at 3

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Before the age of three, children generally have
little comprehension of professional roles. At roughly three years of age, most
children perceive the professionals with whom they are familiar as a cluster of
behaviors and characteristics (for example, doctors are people who wear whit
ecouats and give shots). Children at this stage are still too young to
comprehenend roles in relational or purposive terms, and they have difficulty
comprehending that a single person can fill several roles in relation to
different people (ex. father to children, husband to wife, doctor to patients).</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Kids understanding of roles between 4 and 8

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Between the ages of four and eight, however,
children gradually acquire the ability to comprehend roles in terms of their
purposes and their relationships (doctors help pateients when they are sick).</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Most seven or eight year olds are able to
understand the professional roles with which they are familiar in relevant, if
concrete, terms.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

In
order for a child to achieve a true understanding of the lawyer-child
relationship, 

A

 the child must have both the capacity to
understand the lawyer’s role, and a familiarity with that role. While only the
child can supply the capacity, it is up to the lawyer to ensure the role is
familiar to the child.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

What happens if child lacks developmental capacity to understand lawyer’s role?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Where the child lacks the developmental capacity
to understand his lawyer’s role at any level, the lawyer is freed from her
obligation to communicat that role, for such communication is, by definition, not
“reasonably possible.” And where the role cannot be communicated, it is
probably inappropriate for the lawyer to assume the traditional attorney role,
for the child who cannot understand his lawyer’s role (or his own role in the
relationship) will never be in a position truly to direct the representation.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Talking alone to get child to understand role?

A

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Talking at children, particularly about matters
divorced from their experience, is unlikely to advance children’s
understanding.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
For children truly to understand what it means
to have a lawyer, and what that lawyer does, they need to experience the
process, as particpants and as observers.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Lawyers cannot expect to convey their role
effectively to most children by simply explaining things more clearly. Children
have no context, no relevant experience ot make these explanations make sense.
Indeed, attempts to create a context by description are more likely to mislead
than illuminate, for they must rely on inapt comparisons (who is a lawyer
like?), or confounding elaborations on the unpredictable and chaotic goings-on
of juvenile or family court.</div>

<div>-&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;
Unless a child sees his lawyer in action and has
an opportunity engage in the process of representation as it occurs, he will
lack the expertise to make the cognitive accommodation necessary to perceive
the lawyer’s role for the unique role that it is.</div>

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
What does coutroom observation allow for child?
-          Only in the courtroom can the child observe the costellation of participants, the decision-making process, and the role his lawyer plays in that process. And only in the halls outside the coutroom can the child observe the negotiation process that is often more key than any hearing in determining his fate. The value of coming to court to a child’s understanding of his lawyer’s role is great regardless of which model the lawyer assumes.
26
impact on lawyer of having kid in courtroom?
-          Away from the watchful eye of the client, it is very easy for traditional attorneys, in particular, to modify the child client’s position to one that is easier for the lawyer to swallow. Where, for example, the child has expressed a desire to go home, the lawyer may represent the child’s position to the court (if the child is abset) as a desire to go home “as soon as appropriate” or “as soon as his mother completes drug treatment,” even if such qualifications were never provided by the child.
27
Argument against kid in courtroom? Response?
-          There is considerable disagreement about the advisability of bringing children to court. Most of the concern focuses on the potential harm to children, although there is also administrative resistance among child welfare agencies to the prospect of being responsible for the burdensome and time-consuming process of bringing children to court and taking them home afterwards. The fear for children is that they will be traumatized by the place and by the process. At court, children are forced to come face-to-face with their history of abuse or neglect and with the deep family tensions invokved by the proceeding. Moreover, children are often called upon take sides, against one or both parents.
-          While coming to court can provke considerable anxiety in children, we fool ourselves if we think that avoiding court protects children from those anxieties. Much of the anxiety is created by the existence of the court process (a process whereby a child’s future is determined by a judge) and the issues underlying the court’s involvement (the abuse, neglect, foster care placement, or divorce). Lawyers’ conversations with their cleitns tend to fan those anxieties, whether or not the child appeared in court, because these conversations inevitably (if grounded at all) focus on the court process and the decisions the judge will have to make. Realistically, a child is not shiled from the court process until the court dismisses his case.
28
play for instructing child role?
-          The value of interactive learning can also probably be captured through modeling and role-playing exercises.
29
- Interviewer bias characterizes those interviewers who 
who hold a priori beliefs about the occurrence of certain events and, as a result, mold the interview to maximize disclosures from the interviewee that are consistent with the interviewer’s prior belief’s.
30
Hallmarks of  interviewer bias are
is the single-minded attempt to gather only confirmatory evidence (e.g. testing incompatible hypotheses). Thus, biased interviewers do not ask questions that might provide alterative explanatisonf rot eh allegations. Nor do biased interviewers ask about events that are inconsistent with their hypothesis. And biased interviewers do not challenge the authenticity of the child’s report when it is consistent with their hypothesis. When children provide inconsistent or bizarre evidence, it is either ignored or else interpreted within the framework of the biased interviewer’s initial hypothesis.
31
When questioned by a neutral intervierwe or by an interviewer whose interpreteation was consistent with the activity viewed by the child... However, when the interviewer was biased in a direction that contradicted the activity viewed by the children... 
When questioned by a neutral intervierwe or by an interviewer whose interpreteation was consistent with the activity viewed by the child (an janitor’s interaction with dolls), children’s accounts were both factually correct and consistent with the janitor’s script. However, when the interviewer was biased in a direction that contradicted the activity viewed by the child, those children’s stories conformed to the suggestions or beliefs of the interviewer. Also, children’s answers to interpretive questions (was he doing his job or just being bad) were in agreement with the interviewer’s point of view, as opposed to what actually happened. When asked neutral questions by their parents, the children’s answers remained consistent with the interviewers’ biases.
32
To obtain confirmation of their supsicions, biased interviewers often do not ask children 
open-ended questions such as “what happened?” but instead resort to a barrage of specific questions, many of which are repeated, and many of which are leading. This strategy is problematic because children’s responses to open-ended questions are more accurate than their responses to specific questions. 
33
accuracy in free recall vs specific questions
Children were first asked open-ended questions (tell me what happened), and then more specific questions (where did you hurt yourself or did you hurt your knee). The children were most likely to accurately report the important details inf ree recall (91% accuracy); errors increased when children were asked specific questions (45% accuracy). 
34
why impact of forced-choice questions
-  One of the reasons that children so willing provide answers to specific yes/no or to forced-choice questions even though they may not know the answer is that young children are cooperative: They perceive their adult interviewer as truthful, not deceptive. To comply with a respectful adult, children sometimes attempt to make their answers consistent with what they see as the intent of the questioner rather than consistent with their knowledge of the event 
35
Impact of repeating specific questions when intervieweing kid
Not only does accuracy decrease when children are asked specific questions, but there is increased risk of inaccurate reports when young children are repeatedly asked the same specific questions, either within the same interview or across different interviews. Young children tend to change their answers, perhaps to provide the interviewer with the information that the child perceives he wants 
36
repeated open ended question effect vs repeated closed questions
(Poole study) repeated open-ended questions (what did the man look like) had little effect, positive or negative, on children’s responses. However, on repeated yes/no questions (did the man hurt Melanie), the younger children were more likely to change their rsponses, both within and across sessions. Also, when children were asked a specific question about a detail for which they had no information (what did the man do for a living), many answered with sheer speculations. Furthermore, with repeated questions, they used fewer qualifiers, omitting phrases such as “it might have been,” and consequently they sounded increasingly confident about their statements. IN other words, children will often cooperate by guessing, but after several repetitions, their uncertainty is no longer apparent.
37
For example, in one study, five-year old children received an inoculation from a pediatritcian. One year later, they were interviewed four times about salient details of that visit. Children who were repeatedly interviewed in a neutral,... vs children who were repeatedly given misinformation about some of the salient details
For example, in one study, five-year old children received an inoculation from a pediatritcian. One year later, they were interviewed four times about salient details of that visit. Children who were repeatedly interviewed in a neutral, nonleading manner provided accurate reports about the original medical visit. In contrast, children who were repeatedly given misinformation about some of the salient details incorporated the misleading suggestions into their reports, they also reported nonsuggested but inaccurate events.
38
Does suggestibility increase over course of multiple interviews
Other studies show that when children are repeatedly and suggestively interviewed about false events, assent rates rise for each interview. For example, childrena re more likely to assent to a false event in a third interview than in a second interview.
For example, in the pediatrician study, he children were given suggestions immediately after they had received their inoculation about how much the inoculation had hurt. This suggestive interview had no effect on children’s reports taken one week after the inoculation, presumably because the episode was still fresh in their mind. However, one year later, when the same children were given similar suggestions, these children now routinely underestimate their level of pain and crying as a result of suggestions about how brave and courageous they had been.
- Over time, children’s memory of the original event fades with time, allowing the misinformation to become more easily planted.
39
what reports are most accurate when child interviewing
- Reports that emerge in a child’s first interview with a neutral interviewer are the most accurate. When children are later interviewed about the same event and report new details not mentioned in the first interview, these have a high probability of being inaccurate. 
40
emotional atmospherics in child interivew
Children may give incorrect information to misleading questions about events for which they have no memory if the interviewer creates an emotional tone of accusation. 
41
stereotyp induction child interview
- Stereotype induction is another component of a suggestive interview. For example, if a child is repeatedly told that a person “does bad things” then the child may begin to incorporate this belief into his or her reports.
42
Clumsy Sam Stone study
- In one study, provided animated descriptions of their “clumsy” friend Sam Stone to preschool children. ON a number of occasions, these children were told of Sam’s exploits, which included accidently breaking Barbie dolls or ripping sweaters. Later, the children met Sam Stone, who came to their classroom for a short, accident-free visit. The next day, the teacher showed the children a torn book and a soiled teddy bear. Several weeks later, a number of three- and four-year-old children reported that Sam Stone had been responsible, with some even claiming that they had seem him do this. Children who had not received stereotype induction rarely made this type of error
43
Anatomically detailed dolls purpose
- Anatomically detailed dolls. Techniquest that have been especially designed for interviewing children about sexual abuse may be potentially suggestive. For example, anatomically detailed dolls are commonly used by professionals when interviwering children about spesected sexual abuse. It is thought that the use of dolls overcomes language, memory, and motivational (e.g. embarrassment) problems. However, the existing data indicate that the dolls do not facilitate accurate reporting
44
Guided imagery in child interviews
Thinking about and imagining. Guided imagery is another interviewing technique that is potentially sugesstive. Interviewers sometimes ask children to try to remember or pretend if a certain event occurred, and then to create a mental picture of the event and to think about its details. Because young children sometimes have difficulty distinguishing between memories of actual events and memories of imagined events, when asked to pretend about or imagine certain events, children may later come to report and believe these imagined events.
45
does number of suggestive techniques used matter?
- The number of suggestive techniques used in an actual interview is a function of the degree of the interviewer’s bias. Interviewers who have strong a priori beliefs and who view their role as one of obtaining information to confirm these beliefs will include the most suggestive elements in their interviews. However, when a number of techniques are combined in one interview, these procedures have detrimental effects much larger than seen in studies where only one suggestive technique is used.
46
Is it easy to detect when kids are making things up as a reesult of suggestions? Why?
- It has often been stated that it is easy to detect false reports that are the result of suggestion, because it was thought that children were merely “parroting” the words of their interrogators. However, evidence from the past decade provides no support for this assertion. First, we have found that when children are suggestively interviewed, their subsequent narratives include false reports that were not suggested to them, but that are consistent with the suggestions [Bruck M, Ceci SJ, Francoeur E, Barr RJ. 1995a. “I hardly cried when I got my shot!”: influencing children’s reports about a visit to their pediatrician. Child Dev. 66:193–208]. Second, subjective ratings of children’s reports after suggestive interviewing reveal that these children appear highly credible to trained professionals in the fields of child development, mental health, and forensics; these professionals cannot reliably discriminate between children whose reports are accurate from those whose reports are inaccurate as the result of suggestive interviewing techniques. 
also, - In the study wherein children were repeatedly and suggestively interviewed about true and false events, the children’s narratives of the false events became more embellished and detailed, so that by the third interview, it was impossible to differentiate the true from the false narratives on a number of factors that are generally considered to be markers of good narratives and of autobiographical recall. That is, by the third interview, the false narratives contained the same number of spontaneous statements, details, adjectives, emotional terms, and dialogue statements as did the true narratives.
47
Measures that differentiate true from false stories accurately
Two measuers differentiated the true and false stories. First, children were more likely to repeat the same details across interviews for true than for false narratives. Thus, the true narratives were more consistent than the false narratives. One reason for the difference was the fact that with each retelling, children included more new details in their false than in their true narratirves (the false stories expanded and sometimes changed). Second, for some of the children, with reapted suggestive intervies, the number of aggressive, exaggerated, and fantasitical details increased for false narratives, but not for true narratives.
48
Why do children respond to suggestibility
We hypothesize that a more detailed inspection of children’s responses over time will reflect a more complex condition with a comingling of social (compliance) and cognitive (memory) factors in the emercence of false reports. For example, children may start out knowingly complying to suggestions, but with repeated suggestive interviews, they may come to believe and incorporate the suggestions into their memories. However, depending upon the strength of the false belief, children may eventually come to forget their misreports and thus recant their previous allegations, especially if suggestive interviewing has ceased for a long period.
49
age and suggestibility
- Preschoolers are the most suggestible age group. Preschoolers do differ from older children and adults in their susceptibility to misleading or incorrect post-event information, although these authors estimated a smaller effect size than heretofore assumed
 - It is nonetheless important to point out that concern remains about the reliability of oler children’s testimony when they are suggestive to suggestive interviews. Ample evidence may be cited that children older than six are suggestible about a wide range of events and that adults’ recollections also are impaired by suggestive interviewing techniques.
50
do preschoolers have good memories
- It is clear that children—even preschoolers—are capable of accurately recalling much that is forensically relevant. For example, in many of our own studies, children in the control group conditions recalled events flawlessly. This indicates that the absence of suggestive techniques allows even very young preschoolers to provide highly accurate reports, although they may be sparese in the number of details.
51
what characterizes a good interview
What characterizes many such studies is the neutral tone of the interviewer, the limited use of misleading questions, and the absence of the induction of any motive for the child to make a false report. When such conditions are present, it is a common (although not universal) finding that children are much more immune to suggestive influences, particularly about sexual details.
52
What has biggest impact on reliability of child witnesses?
- In a very real sense, the reliability of young children’s reports has more to do with the skills of the interviewer than to any natural limitations on their memory. Research on this topic has been fast finding its way into courts of law, used by one side or the other to bolster or discredit child witnesses’ testim ony.
53
- I argued that the rececent research on suggestibility was inspired by
highly coercive interviewing techniques in widely publicized cases that are not the norm in child sexual abuse investigations. These techniques include telling children that they have been abused, telling children that a particular person is the abuser, and asking children to imagine details regarding how abuse coud have taken place. Moreover, I argued that the research fails to mirror factors in real-world sexual-abuse cases that reduce the likelihood that false allegations will occur. These factors include the age of the child, children’s reluctance to accuse loved ones of immortal acts, and children’s embarrassment regarding sexual topics
54
- One of the suggestive techniques the researches used was “stereotype induction,” which they analogized to negative statements that adult smight make about an ex-spouse. Research assistants visited each child on four consecutive weeks before Sam Stone came to the preschool, and 
and provided the child with details of 12 different misdeeds that the assistant had purportedly witnessed Sam perform. Sam Stone’s visit was two minutes long, and he did not interact with individual children.
55
- These questions presupposed Sam Stone had 
had ripped the book or soiled the teddy bear, did not give the child an opportunity to deny that he had done so, and asked the child to choose among details of the fictituious events. For example, “Did Sam Stone rip the book with his hands, or did he use scissors?”
- These questions were asked regardfless of whether the child affirmed or denie that Sam Stone had performed any misdeeds. Ten weeks later, all children were interviewed in a nonleading fashion. At that time, 72% of three and four year olds implicated Sam in one or both misdeeds.
56
The rate of false reports among the older preschoolers, who were five to six years of age, was about 
half of that of the younger children. School-age children would be even less likely to succumb to the interviewer's pressures.
57
One of the most consistent findiungs in the suggestibility literature is that preschool children are 
particularly vulernable to suggestive questioning, and preschool children predominate in recent research documenting the unreliability of children’s testimony.
58
- Second, the study was ununusal in that the final interview contained two questions mildly skeptical of the children’s claims. Asking childfren if they saw the events reduced the number of false reports 
by about half. Asking the children, “you didn’t really see him rip the book (or soil the bear), did you? Cut the number by half again. Having been exposed to four trials of stereotype induction and three trials of suggestive questioning, 21% of the three and four year old s (and only 5% of the five and six year olds) maintained that the misdeeds had occurred.
- These percetnages exaggerate the likelihood that a child will accuse a familiar adult of sexual abuse, compared to the likelihodd that a child will accuse a stranger of ripping a book or soiling a teddy bear.
59
The issue is not whether children can be led to make false allegations, 
but whether they are being led by current investigative methods.
60
]- There are large age differences in suggestibility. I am struck by how many experts appear to overlook the truism that just as preschoolers are much more suggestible than school-age children, 
school-age children are much less suggestible than preschoolers
61
children are both suggestible and 
and counter-suggestible. Researchers often fail to test the persistence of their suggestions; Sam Stone is an exception,a nd dramatically reduced the number of false reports.
62
, much of the suggestibility research elicits false narratives from young childfren by 
by telling them that the events occurred (as opposed to merely asking them), and by providing them with details with which they can imagine the events.
63
What had been documented was that inrerviewers were asking very few
open-ended questions, and relying heavily on closed-ended questions (yes-no questions and forced choice questions). Closed-ended questions are often considered leading and I believe they are being overused. - I emphasize the need for structured interview protocols and the potential benefits of rapport building and greater use of open-ended questions as means of increasing information without reducing reliability.
64
All the same, closed-ended questions are far less leading than 
than the kind of questions asked in studies like Sam Stone.
65
- close-ended questions are also 
necessary in some cases. Abused children are often quite reluctant to describe abuse that was painful, shameful, and embarrassing
66
The recent research on preschool children’s suggestibility does not stop with “one strong suggestion.” It is the
the dogged perspistence of coercive interviewers that reliably produces false narratives in young children. The cases that inspired the research involved unrelenting suggestion over longperiods of time by interviewers utterly convinced that abuse had occurred.
67
in determining truthfullness - Judges should keep in mind the importance of the 
child’s age, the suggestive influences at issue, and the relationship of the child to the alleged offender.
68
Advising child client
  • As with any client, the child’s attorney may counsel against the pursuit of a particular position sought by the child. The child’s attorney should recognize that the child may be more susceptible to intimidation and manipulation than some adult clients.
69
What determines a child's ability to determine their position?
  • The child’s ability to contribute to a determination of his or her position is functional, depending upon the particular position and the circumstances prevailing at the time the position must be determined. Therefore, a child may be able to determine some positions in the case but not others.
70
What does lawyer need to be aware of when advising child client?
  • As in any other lawyer/client relationship, the lawyer may express his or her assessment of the case, the best position for the child to take, and the reasons underlying such recommendation. A child, however, may agree with the lawyer for inappropriate reasons. A lawyer must remain aware of the power dynamics inherent in adult/child relationships. Therefore, the lawyer needs to understand what the child knows and what factors are influencing the child’s decision. The lawyer should attempt to determine from the child’s opinion and reasoning what factors have been most influential or have been confusing or glided over by the child when deciding the best time to express his or her assessment of the case.
71
Talking with child therapist and other experts?
  • Consistent with the rules of confidentiality and with sensitivity to the child’s privacy, the lawyer should consult with the child’s therapist and other experts and obtain appropriate records. For example, a child’s therapist may help the child to understand why an expressed position is dangerous, foolish, or not in the child’s best interests. The therapist might also assist the lawyer in understanding the child’s perspective, priorities, and individual needs.
72
Failure to express vs directive to not take position
  • The child’s failure to express a position is distinguishable from a directive that the lawyer not take a position with respect to certain issues.
  • The lawyer should clarify with the child whether the child wants the lawyer to take a position or remain silent with respect to that issue or wants the preference expressed only if the parent or other party is out of the courtroom. The lawyer is then bound by the child’s directive. The position taken by the lawyer should not contradict or undermine other issues about which the child has expressed a preference.
73
How to determine child's best interest?
  • The determination of the child’s legal interests should be based on objective criteria as set forth in the law that are related to the purpose of the proceedings. The criteria should address the child’s specific needs and preferences, the goal of expeditious resolution of the case so the child can remain or return home or be placed in a safe, nurturing, and permanent environment, and the use of the least restrictive or detrimental alternatives available.
  • The lawyer should base the position on objective criteria concerning the child’s needs and interests, and not merely on the lawyer’s personal values, philosophies, and experiences.
74
Does kinship automatically mean good?
  • The lawyer must determine the child’s feelings about the proposed caretaker, however, because familiarity does not automatically confer positive regard.
75
When should you meet with child clieent? Why?
  • Meeting with the child is important before court hearings and case reviews. In addtion, changes in placement, school suspensions, in-patient hospialiations, and othe rsimilar changes warrant meeting again with the child. Such in-person meetings allow the lawyer to explain to the child what is happening, what alternatives might be available, and what will happen next. This also allows the lawyer to assess the child’s circumstances, often leading to a greater understanding of the case, which may lead to more creative solutions in the child’s interest.
76
Older children and record release authorization?
  • Even if it is not required, an older child should be asked to sign authorizations for release of his or her own records, because such a request demonstrates the lawyer’s respect for the client’s authority over information.
77
Determining child presence at trial
  • A child has the right to meaningful participation in the case, which generally includes the child’s presence at significant hearings. The child’s presence underscores for the judge that the child is a real party in interest in the case. 
  • A decision to exclude the child from the hearing should be made based on a particularized determination that the child does not want to attend, is too young to sit through the hearing, would be severely traumatized by such attendance, or for other good reason would be better served by nonattendance. The lawyer should consult the child, therapist, caretaker, or any other knowledgeable person in determining the effect on the child of being present at the hearing.
  • Even a child who is too young to sit through the hearing may benefit from seeing the courtroom and meeting, or at least seeing, the judge who will be making the decisions.
78
Determining child testifying
  • The decision of whether or not the child should testify should include consideration of the child’s need or desire to testify, any repercussions of testifying, the necessity of the child’s direct testimony, the availability of other evidence or hearsay exceptions which may substitute for direct testimony by the child, and the child’s developmental ability to provide direct testimony and withstand possible cross-examination. Ultimately, the child’s attorney is bound by the child’s direction concerning testifying.
  • While testifying is undoubtedly traumatic for many children, it is therapeutic and empowering for others. The decision about the child’s testifying should be made individually, based on circumstances of the individual child and the individual case. The child’s therapist should be consulted with respect to the decision itself and assistance with preparation. In the absence of compelling reasons, a child who has a strong desire to testify should be called to do so. 
79
What to do if child does not want to testify
  • If the child should not wish to testify or would be harmed by being forced to testify, the lawyer shoudl seek a stipulation of the parties not to call the child as a witness or seek a protective order from the court. If the child is compelled to testify, the lawyer should seek to minimize the testimony taekn informally, in chambers, without presence of the parents.
  • The child should know whether the in-chambers testimony will be shared with others, such as parents who might be excluded from chambers, before agreeing to this forum. 
80
Child who participates and fact judge may rule against them. What does child need to be told?
  • The lawyer should also prepare the child for the possibility that the judge may render a decision against the child’s wishes which will not be the child’s fault.
81
Child developmental limitations and testimony
  •  The information a child gives in interviews and during testimony is often misleading because the adults have not understood how to ask children developmentally appropriate questions and how to interpret their answers properly. The child’s attorney must become skilled at recognizing the child’s developmental limitations.
  • The child’s competency to testify, or the reliability of the child’s testimony or out-of-court statements, may be called into question. The child’s attorney should be familiar with the current law and empirical knowledge about children’s competency, memory, and suggestibility and, where appropriate, attempt to establish the competency and reliability of the child.
82
What to do when representation of child client ends?
  • When the representation ends, the child’s lawyer should explain in a developmentally appropriate manner why the representation is ending and how the child can obtain assistance in the future should it become necessary. I is important for there to be closure between the child and the lawyer.