Speech segmentation Flashcards

(27 cards)

1
Q

Jusczyk and Aslin (1995) 7.5 months repetitions of sentences containing 2 target words

A

Tested on individual words. Infants listened longer to words embedded in familiarisation sentences. By 7.5 months, at least some rudimentary ability to detect words when they occur in fluent speech contexts.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

6 possible ways to help segment speech stream

A
  1. Prosodic cues.
  2. Phonotactic regularities.
  3. Allophonic variation.
  4. Isolated words.
  5. Transitional probabilities.
  6. Multiple cues approach.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What are prosodic cues?

A

Rhythm, stress pattern, and intonation of speech. English has trochaic patten (strong-weak stress) so segmentation through high probability that strong syllable belongs to onset of new word.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Jusczyk et al. (1999) prosodic cues familiarisation to individual strong-weak words

A

7.5 month infants listened longer to passages containing words they were exposed to (suggests segmentation). Effect not seen for weak-strong words (but is at 10.5 months).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

2 critiques of prosodic cues

A
  1. Not a perfect cue (is segmentation seem before 7.5 months?).
  2. Not relevant for all languages.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Phonotactic regularities

A

There are patterns as to which sounds can co-occur within a language (eg. [br] at beginning of word, [nt] at end, [vzg] not legal in Englsih). Sensitivity could provide reasonable cue to word boundaries.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

When do we see evidence of sensitivity to phonotactic regularities?

A

About 9 months.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Allophonic variation

A

A phoneme might be pronounced differently at the beginning/ends of words (eg. aspirated vs. non-aspirated).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Jusczyk et al. (1994) allophonic variation ‘nitrates’ vs. ‘night rates’

A

Infants heard examples of ‘nitrates’ vs. ‘night rates’ embedded in continuous speech. Infants listened longer to sentence containing the word they were familiarised to (seem at 10.5 months but not 9 months).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Big problem in speech segmentation

A

Need words to segment and need to segment to get words. Infants need to learn what’s relevant for their language before they can use it for segmentation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Isolated words

A

Might help with early segmentation. Around 9% of words in IDS occur in isolation and tend to be words learnt first. But this method also identifies phrases as words.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Statistical learning

A

Discovery of patterns or structure in the input. Learning is implicit (without instruction or rules). Infants highly sensitive to distributional info in input.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Transitional probabilities

A

Conditional probability of Y given X in sequence XY.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Saffran et al. (1996) familiarised infants to continuous speech of trisyllabic words strung together in random order

A

8-month infants. Test word vs. non-word vs. part-word (syllable sometimes follows). Looked longer to near novel stimulus (non-word or part-word). Suggests babies are sensitive to transitional probabilities.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Factors influencing direction of preference for novelty vs. familiarisation

A
  1. Length of familiarisation.
  2. Meaningfulness of stimuli.
    Need pre-registration.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Gambel and Yang (2004) computer model of English utterances and use TP to identify word boundaries

A

Only 41% were real English words. 77% of words were missed. TP not a sufficient cue to identify words from continuous speech on its own.

17
Q

Multiple cues

A

Use all possible cues (prosody, phonotactic, allophonic, isolated words, TPs). Once some words identified, they can pick up common patterns that mark beginning and ends of words.

18
Q

Thiessen and Saffran (2003) artificial speech stream with colliding cues

A

7 months-olds: more attention to TPs to segment speech.
2. 9 month-olds: prioritised stress cues over TPs.

Developmental change.

19
Q

Another form of conditional probability

A

Meaning predicting form. Infant regularly hears ‘mummy’ in speech stream and mother is generally present at the same time. Link the 2 together (correlations).

20
Q

Saffran (2020) statistical learning process in language acquisition

A

Ability to track probabilistic relationships between different aspects of language. Input-driven approach.

21
Q

2 challenges for statistical learning

A
  1. Do infants actually use it?
  2. Rules? Could abstractions about patterns be generalised to novel elements. Language is structured, not just statistical.
22
Q

Graf Estes et al. (2007)

A

17 month infants. Exposed to continuous speech stream with TP as only clue to word boundaries. Then, habituated to object-label association (objects paired with words and nonwords).

Test: switch or same object-label pair. Switch condition led to longer looking time for words but not nonwords or partwords. Suggests that words treated as linguistic units that can be mapped to meanings.

23
Q

Marcus et al. (1997) what else is needed in addition to statistical learning?

A

Need an abstract rule learning mechanism. Necessary to generalise to new exemplars, which is critical to language learning.

24
Q

Marcus et al. (1999) familiarised to different patterns ABA or ABB and then tested on preferential listening for same or novel structure

A

Infants show a looking-time preference for novel sentences. Claimed that this is evidence for a rule-learning system that operates alongside statistical learning.

25
2 challenges to language rule-learning system
1. Lack of specificity (not language- or species-specific). A general purpose system instead. 2. No need for a separate system - perhaps set of experience enough for generalisation.
26
Gerken (2006) diagonal AAB (abstract rule) vs. just having the same ending (low-level regularity) to test the basis of generalisation
9 months. Diagonal condition generalised to AAB but column condition only generalised to 'di'. Challenges idea of a dedicated abstract rule system. Instead, learning is an interaction between perceptual salience and consistency of contextual cues (determine generalisation). Single mechanism. Learning accomplished without instruction through structured input exposure.
27
Geambasu et al. replication of Marcus et al. (1999)
Failed to replicate. No difference in looking time between test patterns. Not support hypothesis that infants are able to learn abstract 'algebraic' rules and apply them to novel input.