lecture 12 - Introduction to memory systems - memory joints Flashcards
The “joints” of memory
“Carving nature at its joints” Plato
* Complex system needs classification to be
better understood and described
* Break complex system into components,
but try to do this in a natural, non-
arbitrary way
* Goal: Establish taxonomy of memory types
that make scientific sense
how do we learn about memory
By thinking
about it, via
introspection
Via empirical
research and
theory-
building
Cycles: We consider
how findings from
different methods fit
together or contradict
and re-assess theories
continuously
memory processes
- Encoding – entering information into
the system - Storage – holding information statically
- Retrieval – getting information out of
the system - Let’s apply these terms to a basic
model of memory, a starting point in
our taxonomy
An information-Processing approach
- Intuitive; attributed to Broadbent
- Emphasizes memory as a system for internally preserving
information from the environment - Boxes represent permanent, universal structures. Processes that
control or act upon contents might be more flexible and variable. - Arrows represent how the information moves from one system to
another
environment —>(x2 arrows) sensory memory —-> (x2 arrows) short term memory —-> (x1 arrow) long term memory
Environment
- Contains information that we might want to
know/preserve - It’s outside the memory system, but you could
consider this “storage” (O’Regan, 1992)
Sensory memory
- Brief storage of information within a specific perceptual modality (e.g.,
audition, vision, etc.) - Very recent memories retain high sensory fidelity for a lot of
information, closely linked to perceptual systems - Processes: Information from environment encoded into sensory
memory
Short-term memory
- Retention of small amounts of information over periods of a
few seconds - Processes: Some information stored in sensory memory is
encoded into short-term memory
Long-term memory
- System(s) for holding information over long periods of time
- Some information stored in short-term memory is encoded into long-term
memory - When you need to use a piece of knowledge, you retrieve it
- Information loss
- Not all that is remembered briefly is transferred to long-term memory
Why these distinctions?
- Our goal: assess these distinctions to figure out what we think of
these distinctions. Are these memory’s natural “joints”? - Today: let’s consider short-term and long-term memory
- Evidence that time retained might define a natural “joint” in the
memory system comes from amnesia case
Henry Molaison (HM)
Had surgery removing medial temporal lobes to treat severe epilepsy
- After surgery meant to treat epilepsy, HM lost:
- Some long-term memories
- Memory strategies did not help
- Ability to retain new memories (beyond immediately)
- Aspects of HM’s cognition that were spared:
- Most sensory functions
- Normal overall IQ (some components of test were
affected) - Conversations (debateable)
- Selective attention
- Immediate memory (unless he was distracted)
- Procedural memory
- Importance of case: dissociates some kinds of
memory from others
Dissociation
But is this
dissociation really to do with time?
- HM could think: he could hold a
conversation, he could briefly remember
novel information and briefly use it, he
could control what he did - He could remember information for
periods of seconds (short-term
memory?) - HM could not explicitly learn new
information, or retrieve long-term
memories formed over several years prior
to his surgery (long-term memory) - This represents a dissociation between STM
and LTM - Damage to medial temporal lobe affects (some)
long-term but not short-term memory - Double dissociation occurs if a different
variable affects memory at short term only
HM’s deficits dissociation proposed
Some evidence shows that patients with similar amnesia can encode new memories, and
can remember them for long periods as long as they do nothing in between learning and
testing (Cowan, Beschin, & della Sala, 2004)
How can we distinguish between failed storage of LTM, versus failed retrieval from LTM?
* Possible to “forget” but recall later?
* Consider superior autobiographical memory
issues between STM AND LTM
Why this information processing
scheme cannot be right
- It certainly isn’t complete
- Testing it: can we isolate sensory, short-
term, or long-term memory in a test? - Short-term memory is usually
tested with serial recall of a novel
verbal list. - Patients like HM can recall these
lists. - But what about more novel
information?
Can we isolate STM or LTM in a single test?
- Most memory tests focus on verbal information
- Why?
- Would that isolate short-term memory?
- The amnesic patients with medial-
temporal lobe damage (red lines) in
these reports were tested on
memory for novel visual objects - Clear deficits even at short delays
- Conclusion: maybe ”short-term”
memory is not normal in these
From Ranganath & Blumenfeld, 2005
patients after al
graphs in notes
Simple information-processing
scheme is not right
- It is incomplete at best
- What about the “procedural” memory that
HM was quite good at? Isn’t that “long-
term”? - Patients with medial-temporal lobe
lesions fail visual short-term memory
tests - Could mean that additional “joints”
needed in short-term memory - Could mean that thinking about
memory in terms of time is not a good
“joint”
what is memory
- Schacter (2001) described the 7 sins of memory and accepts the sins are the necessary consequences of the virtues that make our memories rich and flexible.
- We are good at coping with forgetting by using knowledge to reconstruct partial memories
- Our falliable memories play an crucial part in our ability to function independently in our complex world
Clive wearing has much of his memory capacity destroyed by diease
Why do we need memory?
- Clive wearing = talented musician, master of a major london choir, singer and asked to sing for pope in papal visit to london. 1985 got brain infection from herpes simplex virus that caused encephalitis and inflammation of the brain that can be fatal. When he recovered consciousness he was amnesic and was unable to store info for periods longer than seconds. His interpretation was that he had just discovered consciousness and would announce that to any visitor and write it in his notebook each time crossing out the previous line and writing “I have now recovered consciousness” or “consciousness has now finally been recovered,” an activity that continued for many, many years.
- Clive knew who he was and could about his early life but in little detail. He knew he went to cambridge uni for 4 years but could not recognise a photo of his college.
- He could remember, although somewhat vaguely, important events in his life such as directing and conducting the first modern performance of Handel’s Messiah using original instruments in an appropriate period setting, and could talk intelligently about the historical development of the role of the musical conductor. However, even this selected knowledge was sketchy; he had written a book on the early composer Lassus, but could not recall any of the content. Asked who had written Romeo and Juliet, Clive did not know. He had remarried, but could not remember this. However, he did greet his new wife with enormous enthusiasm every time she appeared, even though she might only have been out of the room for a few minutes; every time declaring that he had just recovered consciousness.
- He was incapacitated by his amnesia - he could not read a book or follow a tv show because he forgot what had happened before. If he left his hospital room he was lost.
- He was locked into a permanent present, something he described as “hell on earth.” “It’s like being dead—all the bloody time!”
- His memory of music was unimpaired, he could conduct his choir and could read a score of a song and play the keyboard and sing.
For a brief moment he appeared to return to his old self, only to feel wretched when he stopped playing. Over 20 years later, Clive is still just as densely amnesic but now appears to have come to terms with his terrible affliction and is calmer and less distressed.
One memory or many?
- Clive had damage to a range of brain areas so had problems beyond his amnesia
- The fact clives musical memory and skills are unimpaired suggests memory is not a single simple system.
- Other studies have shown that densely amnesic patients can repeat back a telephone number, suggesting preserved immediate memory, and that they can learn motor skills at a normal rate.
- Amnesic patients are capable of a number of types of learning, demonstrating this by improved performance, even though they do not remember the learning experience and typicay deny having encountered the situation before.
The evidence suggests that rather than a single global memory system, the picture is more complex
Theories, maps and models
- In 1950s many people though theories should look like theories from physics
- Clark Hull studied the learning behaviour of white rats and used the results to develop a general theory of learning to predict learning behavior in both rats and humans using postulates and equations inspired by Newton’s work (Hull, 1943).
- Hull’s great rival, Tolman (1948) thought of rats as forming ‘cognitive maps’ internal representations of their environment that were acquired as a result of active exploration.
- The controversy rumbled on from the 1930s to the 1950s, and then was abandoned quite suddenly. Both sides found that they had to assume some kind of representation that went beyond the simple association between stimuli impinging on the rat and its learned behavior, but neither seemed to have a solution to the problem of how these could be investigated.
- Broad view of theory = theories are essentially like maps - they summarise our knowledge in a simple and structured way that helps us to understand what is known.
- A good theory helps us ask new questions and that helps us find out more about the topic we are mapping.The nature of the theory will depend on the questions we want to answer
- In the case of psychological theories, different theories will operate at different levels of explanation and focus on different issues.
Reductionism - the view that all scientific explanations hsould aim to be based on a lower level of analysis. Psychology in terms of physiology, physiology in terms of chemistry, and chemistry in terms of physics.
How can we study memory?
- The attempt to understand human memory extends at least as far back as Aristotle, and forms one of the classic questions within the philosophy of mind, although without reaching any firm conclusions.
- Limitation of a purely philosophical approach to the understanding of memory in particular and the mind in general = its reliance on introspection the capcity to reflect and report our ongoing thoughts. These are not unimportant but are not a reliable indication of the way our minds work, for two principal reasons.
- 1 -people differ in what they appear to experience in a given situation; does memory depend on visual imagery, and if not, why do some of us experience it?
- 2 - we are only consciously aware of a relatively small proportion of the mechanisms underpinning our mental life
- While there are still important issues addressed by the philosophy of mind, it is now generally acknowledged these can best be pursued in collaboration with a scientific approach based on empirical evidence.
- Sir Francis Galton found that ‘eminent men’ descriptions of the vividness of their memories after being asked to reflect on their breakfast tavle from that morning varied widely, but this did not affect their accuracy, suggesting a nonconscious process at play. This raises questions about whether people experience memory differently or simply describe it differently. The challenge is to move beyond introspection to better understand memory system
- An answer to this started to develop in Germany in the latter half of the 19th century. It was concerned initially with the discipline of psychophysics, an attempt to systematically map the relationship between physical stimuli such as brightness and loudness onto their perceived magnitude.
- Despite success in linking physical stimuli to the psychological experience of participants, capacities such as learning and memory were initially regarded as unsuitable for experimental study.
- This view was dramatically overturned by a German philosopher Herman Ebbinghaus who conducted an intensive series of experiments on himself over a two year period, showing that it was indeed possible to plot systematic relationships between the conditions of learning and the amount learned. Having published this, the first classic book on the science of memory (Ebbinghaus, 1885), he moved on to study color vision, intelligence, and a range of other questions in the newly developing field of experimental psychology.
- Ebbinghaus started by making the experimental situation simpler, creating material that had no meaning but could be learned and reported verbally. He invented nonsense syllables, which are consonant-vowel-consonant nonwords like zug, pij, and tev. He used himself as the subject, keeping the room, time of day, and fast presentation consistent to prevent finding meaning in the stimuli. Ebbinghaus established key learning principles and the classic forgetting curve, forming the basis for further work in this field.
- The Ebbinghaus tradition developed mainly in the US, focusing on how new learning interacted with existing knowledge. Results were seen as associations between stimuli and responses, often using nonsense syllables. This verbal learning approach grew from the 1930s to the 1960s, leading to the creation of The Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, which later became The Journal of Memory and Language.
Verbal learning - a term applied to an approach to memory that relies principally on the learning of lists of words and nonsense syllables
how many kinds of memory
- Shift from Single to Multiple Memory Systems
Early psychology emphasized a single memory system based on stimulus–response associations.
Cognitive psychology reframed memory as involving multiple systems:
➤ Sensory memory
➤ Short-term memory (STM)
➤ Long-term memory (LTM) - The Modal Model (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968)
Became a widely accepted model of memory in the 1960s.
Known as the modal model because it represented the dominant view of the time.
Core structure:
Environmental input enters sensory memory
Information passes to short-term memory
Some of this is encoded into long-term memory
Figure 1.2 (referenced in the text) shows this linear flow model of memory.
3. Challenges to the Modal Model
Critics argued “memory stores” are too static.
Alternative view: unitary memory system driven by common processes (e.g., Nairne, 1990, 2002).
Observations of task similarities across memory types suggest shared mechanisms.
Authors’ stance:
Both structures (stores) and processes (e.g., encoding, retrieval) matter.
Analogy: Just like brain research requires both anatomy and physiology, memory research needs both static and dynamic perspectives.
4. Bidirectional Flow of Information
The modal model implies one-way flow (environment → STM → LTM).
But research shows information flows both ways:
LTM influences attention and perception.
Our existing knowledge shapes what we notice and remember.
Example:
A football fan watching a match perceives and remembers specific plays more accurately than someone unfamiliar with the sport.
5. Memory System Components
Memory Type Description
Sensory Memory Interface between perception and memory. Short-lived, high capacity.
Short-Term Memory Temporary holding system for small amounts of info.
Long-Term Memory Durable store of knowledge and past experiences.
Each system is subdivided further (e.g., working memory is a form of STM).
Not just sequential—components interact and influence one another.
6. Structure of the Book
Begins with sensory memory (briefly, as it’s closer to perception than memory).
Moves to short-term & working memory.
Ends with a preliminary look at long-term memory
sensory memory
🔥 1. Sparkler Effect: Perception of Visual Continuity
Moving a sparkler in a dark room leaves a fading visual trail.
Suggests brief visual storage—a form of memory allowing continuity.
Movies rely on this: static frames + brief gaps = perceived motion due to visual persistence.
👁️ 2. Iconic Memory (Visual Sensory Memory)
📌 Definition:
Coined by Neisser (1967)—stores visual information very briefly.
Basis for the illusion of continuity in vision.
🧪 Sperling’s Experiments (1960, 1963):
Task Observation
12-letter grid shown briefly Participants could recall only 4–5 items.
Partial report technique (cue tone after display) Participants likely encoded more than they could report.
Recall depends on cue delay Longer delay = worse recall → info fades quickly.
💡 Masking Effects (Sperling, 1963; Turvey, 1973):
Type Mechanism Stage of Processing
Brightness masking Light disrupts trace; stronger with same eye → early, retinal level
Pattern masking Similar visual patterns overwrite info; works across eyes → later visual processing
🔄 These forms of interference demonstrate how fragile and short-lived iconic memory is.
🎯 Function of Iconic Memory:
Helps process the complex visual environment.
Acts as a buffer to build a coherent scene before detailed processing.
Bridges perceptual gaps—e.g., why we see films as continuous rather than a sequence of frames.
🔊 3. Echoic Memory (Auditory Sensory Memory)
📌 Definition:
Also coined by Neisser (1967).
Stores brief auditory information for a short duration.
🧪 Auditory Serial Recall (Murdock, 1967):
Presentation Error Pattern
Visual Errors increase toward the end of the list.
Auditory Recency effect: last 1–2 items are remembered better.
Disrupting with a spoken suffix (e.g., “recall”) removes the recency effect.
Non-speech sounds (e.g., buzzer) do not disrupt recall.
📦 Crowder & Morton (1969): Precategorical Acoustic Store
Echoic memory = a buffer before sounds are processed for meaning.
Effectiveness depends on suffix type—supports a sensory (not semantic) mechanism.
⚖️ Controversy: Memory vs. Perception?
Viewpoint Argument
Memory view Echoic memory is a temporary auditory store aiding recall.
Perception view Effects may arise from ongoing speech perception, not memory storage.
📚 Ongoing debate: is echoic memory best understood as memory or as a perceptual mechanism?
🧩 Key Implications:
Iconic and echoic memory help bridge gaps in sensory input.
Serve as entry points to short-term memory.
Understanding these systems is essential to models of perception and memory
short term and working memory
📦 1. Short-Term Memory (STM)
✅ Definition:
Theory-neutral term.
Refers to temporary storage of small amounts of information over brief periods.
🧠 Key Features:
Not tied to a specific mechanism—leaves open how storage occurs.
STM tasks often involve long-term memory (LTM) support (e.g., chunking, familiarity).
Commonly studied using verbal materials, but also relevant for:
Visual & spatial info ✅
Smell & touch ❌ (less studied)
🔄 Rehearsal:
Verbal rehearsal often used, even with non-verbal stimuli, to maintain performance.
🧰 2. Working Memory (WM)
✅ Definition:
A system for temporary storage & manipulation of information.
Essential for complex cognitive tasks like:
Reasoning
Learning
Comprehension
🧩 Key Assumptions:
Provides a mental workspace.
Tightly linked to attention.
Draws from both STM and LTM resources.
🧠 3. Theoretical Models of WM
Feature Description
Diverse models Theorists tailor models to their research interests.
Common assumption WM integrates memory and attention mechanisms.
Some models emphasize attention over memory.
🧠 4. Baddeley & Hitch’s Multicomponent Model (1974)
🔍 Purpose:
To bridge psychological and neuropsychological research on STM.
To explain WM’s role in real-world cognition (e.g., problem-solving, comprehension).
🏗️ Impact:
Remains highly influential over decades (updated by Baddeley, 2007).
Forms the core focus of Chapter 4.
📚 Summary Table
Concept STM WM
Focus Passive storage Active maintenance + manipulation
Duration Brief Brief, but task-dependent
Contents Small amounts of info Task-relevant info (verbal, visual, spatial)
Associated with Memory Memory + Attention
Key Model Varies Baddeley & Hitch (1974) Multicomponent Mode
long-term memory
We shall use the classification of long-term memory proposed by Squire (1992). As shown in Figure 1.6, this classification makes a broad distinction between explicit or declarative memory and implicit or nondeclarative memory. Explicit memory refers to situations that we would generally think of as involving memory, both for specific events, such as meeting a friend unexpectedly on holiday last year, and remembering facts or information about the world, for example the meaning of the word testify or the color of a ripe banana. Implicit memory refers to situations in which some form of learning has occurred, but which is reflected in performance rather than through overt remembering, riding a bicycle for example or reading a friend’s handwriting more easily because we have encountered it frequently in the past